Effects of the orientation of moving objects on the perception of streaming/bouncing motion displays
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] S. Shimojo,et al. When Sound Affects Vision: Effects of Auditory Grouping on Visual Motion Perception , 2001, Psychological science.
[2] E. Brenner,et al. Continuous Use of Perceived Velocity While Hitting Running Spiders , 1997 .
[3] D. G. Albrecht,et al. Motion direction signals in the primary visual cortex of cat and monkey. , 2001, Visual neuroscience.
[4] P Cavanagh,et al. Attention-based motion perception. , 1992, Science.
[5] Jean Lorenceau,et al. Perceptual grouping in the Ternus display: evidence for an `association field' in apparent motion , 2002, Vision Research.
[6] Streaming and bouncing: observations on motion defined objects , 2000, Clinical & experimental ophthalmology.
[7] M Kubovy,et al. The emergence of visual objects in space-time. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[8] U. Polat,et al. Elongated physiological summation pools in the human visual cortex 1 Preliminary results were reported at the 1996 Annual Meeting of the Society for Neuroscience: [50]. 1 , 1998, Vision Research.
[9] Z L Lu,et al. Three-systems theory of human visual motion perception: review and update. , 2001, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[10] S P McKee,et al. Stimulus configuration determines the detectability of motion signals in noise. , 2000, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[11] E H Adelson,et al. Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[12] Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al. Spatial summation and its interaction with the temporal integration mechanism in human motion perception , 1994, Vision Research.
[13] Ken Nakayama,et al. Serial and parallel processing of visual feature conjunctions , 1986, Nature.
[14] S. Fawcett,et al. Attention Facilitates Sequential Recruitment in the Perception of Motion Direction , 1997 .
[15] S Shimojo,et al. Postcoincidence trajectory duration affects motion event perception , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.
[16] Stuart Anstis,et al. Entrained path deflection in apparent motion , 1986, Vision Research.
[17] S. McKee,et al. Detecting a trajectory embedded in random-direction motion noise , 1995, Vision Research.
[18] David J. Field,et al. Contour integration by the human visual system: Evidence for a local “association field” , 1993, Vision Research.
[19] Charles Spence,et al. Bouncing or streaming? Exploring the influence of auditory cues on the interpretation of ambiguous visual motion , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[20] Jon Driver,et al. Visual search for a conjunction of movement and form is parallel , 1988, Nature.
[21] Shinsuke Shimojo,et al. Beyond perceptual modality: Auditory effects on visual perception , 2001 .
[22] R. Sekuler,et al. Sound alters visual motion perception , 1997, Nature.
[23] O. Andreassen,et al. Mice Deficient in Cellular Glutathione Peroxidase Show Increased Vulnerability to Malonate, 3-Nitropropionic Acid, and 1-Methyl-4-Phenyl-1,2,5,6-Tetrahydropyridine , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[24] Philippe G. Schyns,et al. Attention enhances feature integration , 2003, Vision Research.
[25] J J Koenderink,et al. Effects of element orientation on apparent motion perception , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.
[26] D. Sagi,et al. Vision outside the focus of attention , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.
[27] M. Hallett,et al. Neural correlates of cross-modal binding , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[28] Ken Nakayama,et al. Attentional requirements in a ‘preattentive’ feature search task , 1997, Nature.
[29] A. Treisman,et al. A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.
[30] N. Troje,et al. Audiovisual phenomenal causality , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.
[31] Katsumi Watanabe,et al. Crossmodal Interaction in Humans , 2001 .
[32] B I Bertenthal,et al. Directional Bias in the Perception of Translating Patterns , 1993, Perception.
[33] S. Sumi. Bounce effect seen in opposite motions , 1995 .
[34] S. Yantis,et al. Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: evidence from visual search. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[35] J R Pomerantz,et al. Models of illusory pausing and sticking. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[36] D Jancke,et al. Orientation Formed by a Spot's Trajectory: A Two-Dimensional Population Approach in Primary Visual Cortex , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[37] E. Adelson,et al. Phenomenal coherence of moving visual patterns , 1982, Nature.
[38] Jean Lorenceau,et al. Orientation dependent modulation of apparent speed: psychophysical evidence , 2002, Vision Research.
[39] Z W Pylyshyn,et al. Tracking multiple independent targets: evidence for a parallel tracking mechanism. , 1988, Spatial vision.
[40] D. Lewkowicz,et al. Sound induces perceptual reorganization of an ambiguous motion display in human infants , 2003 .
[41] Wilson S. Geisler,et al. Motion streaks provide a spatial code for motion direction , 1999, Nature.
[42] S Shimojo,et al. Attentional Modulation in Perception of Visual Motion Events , 1998, Perception.
[43] Ronald A. Rensink,et al. Change-blindness as a result of ‘mudsplashes’ , 1999, Nature.
[44] S. Shimojo,et al. Sensory modalities are not separate modalities: plasticity and interactions , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[45] Jun Saiki,et al. Spatiotemporal characteristics of dynamic feature binding in visual working memory , 2003, Vision Research.
[46] Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al. Limits of attentive tracking reveal temporal properties of attention , 2000, Vision Research.
[47] Jun Saiki,et al. Feature binding in object-file representations of multiple moving items. , 2003, Journal of vision.