Realism as a retrieval cue: Evidence for concreteness‐specific effects of realistic, schematic, and verbal components of visualizations on learning and testing
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Thomas Huk,et al. Who benefits from learning with 3D models? the case of spatial ability , 2006, J. Comput. Assist. Learn..
[2] H. Roediger,et al. Semantic specificity in cued recall , 1980, Memory & cognition.
[3] G. Yovel,et al. Learning faces as concepts rather than percepts improves face recognition. , 2019, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[4] H. Smallman,et al. Choosing and using geospatial displays: effects of design on performance and metacognition. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.
[5] Fred Paas,et al. Comparing apples and oranges? A critical look at research on learning from statics versus animations , 2016, Comput. Educ..
[6] Katharina Scheiter,et al. Learning with dynamic and static visualizations: Realistic details only benefit learners with high visuospatial abilities , 2014, Comput. Hum. Behav..
[7] Dorothy M. Chun,et al. SUPPORTING VISUAL AND VERBAL LEARNING PREFERENCES IN A SECOND LANGUAGE MULTIMEDIA LEARNING ENVIRONMENT , 1998 .
[8] John Sweller,et al. Instructional Visualizations, Cognitive Load Theory, and Visuospatial Processing , 2019, Visuospatial Processing for Education in Health and Natural Sciences.
[9] F. Paas,et al. Cognitive Architecture and Instructional Design: 20 Years Later , 2019, Educational Psychology Review.
[10] T. Höffler. Spatial Ability: Its Influence on Learning with Visualizations—a Meta-Analytic Review , 2010 .
[11] H. Ashida,et al. Effects of detailed illustrations on science learning: an eye-tracking study , 2017 .
[12] Endel Tulving,et al. Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory. , 1973 .
[13] Fred Paas,et al. Interactive and dynamic visualizations in teaching and learning of anatomy: a cognitive load perspective. , 2005, Anatomical record. Part B, New anatomist.
[14] Yvonne Kammerer,et al. The effects of realism in learning with dynamic visualizations , 2009 .
[15] W. Schnotz,et al. The role of decorative pictures in learning , 2013 .
[16] E. Tulving,et al. Effectiveness of retrieval cues in memory for words. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.
[17] Günter Daniel Rey,et al. Subjective cognitive load surveys lead to divergent results for interactive learning media , 2020, Human Behavior and Emerging Technologies.
[18] Joachim Reinwein,et al. A second look at Dwyer's studies by means of meta-analysis: the effects of pictorial realism on text comprehension and vocabulary , 1997 .
[19] Günter Daniel Rey,et al. The realism paradox: Realism can act as a form of signaling despite being associated with cognitive load , 2020 .
[20] Arzu Çöltekin,et al. Toward optimizing the design of virtual environments for route learning: empirically assessing the effects of changing levels of realism on memory , 2019, Int. J. Digit. Earth.
[21] Jared Danielson,et al. Computer-based versus paper-based testing: Investigating testing mode with cognitive load and scratch paper use , 2017, Comput. Hum. Behav..
[22] Harvey S. Smallman,et al. The Use of 2D and 3D Displays for Shape-Understanding versus Relative-Position Tasks , 2001, Hum. Factors.
[23] K. Salter,et al. A monte carlo study of the f test and three tests based on ranks of treatment effects in randomized block designs , 1984 .
[24] Francis M. Dwyer,et al. The Relative Effectiveness of Varied Visual Illustrations in Complementing Programed Instruction , 1967 .
[25] Eric N. Wiebe,et al. Visual Representations of DNA Replication: Middle Grades Students’ Perceptions and Interpretations , 2005 .
[26] Günter Daniel Rey,et al. Realistic details in visualizations require color cues to foster retention , 2018, Comput. Educ..
[27] Daniel M. Belenky,et al. The Effects of Idealized and Grounded Materials on Learning, Transfer, and Interest: An Organizing Framework for Categorizing External Knowledge Representations , 2014 .
[28] Detlev Leutner,et al. Visualizers versus verbalizers : Effects of cognitive style on learning with texts and pictures e An eye-tracking study , 2016 .
[29] Mary Hegarty,et al. The Cognitive Science of Visual-Spatial Displays: Implications for Design , 2011, Top. Cogn. Sci..
[30] Harvey S. Smallman,et al. Naive Realism: Misplaced Faith in Realistic Displays , 2005 .
[31] Ko-JanuchtaMarta,et al. Visualizers versus verbalizers , 2017 .
[32] E. Erdfelder,et al. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses , 2009, Behavior research methods.
[33] Slava Kalyuga,et al. Managing split-attention and redundancy in multimedia instruction , 1999 .
[34] M. Kleinknecht,et al. Videos oder Texte in der Lehrerbildung? Effekte unterschiedlicher Medien auf die kognitive Belastung und die motivational-emotionalen Prozesse beim Lernen mit Fällen , 2015 .
[35] Francis M. Dwyer,et al. The Effects of Prior Knowledge, Presentation Mode, and Visual Realism on Student Achievement. , 1984 .
[36] F. Paas,et al. Cognitive Architecture and Instructional Design , 1998 .
[37] Mary Hegarty,et al. Naïve Cartography: How Intuitions about Display Configuration Can Hurt Performance , 2009, Cartogr. Int. J. Geogr. Inf. Geovisualization.
[38] Wolfgang Schnotz,et al. The Cambridge Handbook of Multimedia Learning: An Integrated Model of Text and Picture Comprehension , 2005 .
[39] Francis M. Dwyer,et al. THE EFFECT OF VARYING THE AMOUNT OF REALISTIC DETAIL IN VISUAL ILLUSTRATIONS DESIGNED TO COMPLEMENT PROGRAMMED INSTRUCTION , 1969 .