Neural system for controlling the contents of object working memory in humans.

Working memory (WM), the active maintenance of currently relevant information, is a flexible system allowing for fast and frequent goal-directed changes of rehearsed information. Successful WM maintenance prevents interference from distracting stimuli while allowing new task-relevant information to update the contents of WM. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging to show that when WM contents were updated, regardless of stimulus type (faces or houses), a frontoparietal network showed transient increases in activation. Some of these regions are highly similar to those identified in studies of shifting attention, supporting the idea that updating WM involves a change in the attentional priority afforded to the current perceptual input. A region within the mid-ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, near the junction of the inferior frontal sulcus and precentral sulcus (inferior frontal junction), that has previously been implicated in cognitive control, demonstrated transient increases in activity during updating as well as sustained maintenance activity. A more anterior prefrontal region, middle frontal gyrus, previously implicated in protecting the contents of WM from interfering stimuli during maintenance, demonstrated transient increases in activity during updating. The current study suggests that updating WM results from a combination of increased attention to the visual stimulus and a change in the system's interference protection state.

[1]  R H Logie,et al.  Counting on working memory in arithmetic problem solving , 1994, Memory & cognition.

[2]  K. Shapiro,et al.  Attention to visual pattern information produces the attentional blink in rapid serial visual presentation. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[4]  M. Potter,et al.  A two-stage model for multiple target detection in rapid serial visual presentation. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[5]  R W Cox,et al.  AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. , 1996, Computers and biomedical research, an international journal.

[6]  E. Vogel,et al.  Word meanings can be accessed but not reported during the attentional blink , 1996, Nature.

[7]  C. Degueldre,et al.  Regional brain activity during working memory tasks. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[8]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Planning and Spatial Working Memory: a Positron Emission Tomography Study in Humans , 1996, The European journal of neuroscience.

[9]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task , 1997, Nature.

[10]  E. Vogel,et al.  Electrophysiological Evidence for a Postperceptual Locus of Suppression during the Attentional Blink Time-based Attention and the Attentional Blink , 1998 .

[11]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Functional MRI studies of spatial and nonspatial working memory. , 1998, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[12]  C. Degueldre,et al.  The neural correlates of updating information in verbal working memory. , 1999, Memory.

[13]  B. Postle,et al.  Maintenance versus Manipulation of Information Held in Working Memory: An Event-Related fMRI Study , 1999, Brain and Cognition.

[14]  P. Boesiger,et al.  SENSE: Sensitivity encoding for fast MRI , 1999, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[15]  G. Hitch,et al.  Separate roles for executive and phonological components of working memory in mental arithmetic , 2000, Memory & cognition.

[16]  A. Dove,et al.  Prefrontal cortex activation in task switching: an event-related fMRI study. , 2000, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[17]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. , 2000, Annual review of neuroscience.

[18]  Clayton E. Curtis,et al.  Behavioral and neurophysiological correlates of episodic coding, proactive interference, and list length effects in a running span verbal working memory task , 2001, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[19]  H. Fukuyama,et al.  Dissociable mechanisms of attentional control within the human prefrontal cortex. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[20]  J. Jonides,et al.  Overlapping mechanisms of attention and spatial working memory , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[21]  Marcia K. Johnson,et al.  Neuroimaging a Single Thought: Dorsolateral PFC Activity Associated with Refreshing Just-Activated Information , 2002, NeuroImage.

[22]  S. Yantis,et al.  Transient neural activity in human parietal cortex during spatial attention shifts , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[23]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[24]  R. Passingham,et al.  Active maintenance in prefrontal area 46 creates distractor-resistant memory , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[25]  Jeremy R. Reynolds,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Transient and Sustained Cognitive Control during Task Switching , 2003, Neuron.

[26]  S. Yantis,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of feature-based attentional control. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[27]  J. Duncan,et al.  Encoding Strategies Dissociate Prefrontal Activity from Working Memory Demand , 2003, Neuron.

[28]  Randy L. Buckner,et al.  Mixed blocked/event-related designs separate transient and sustained activity in fMRI , 2003, NeuroImage.

[29]  Martin Lauritzen,et al.  Context sensitivity of activity-dependent increases in cerebral blood flow , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[30]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Neuronal Activity in the Lateral Intraparietal Area and Spatial Attention , 2003, Science.

[31]  Pilar Andrés,et al.  Directed forgetting in working memory: Age‐related differences , 2004, Memory.

[32]  Jan Derrfuss,et al.  Cognitive control in the posterior frontolateral cortex: evidence from common activations in task coordination, interference control, and working memory , 2004, NeuroImage.

[33]  Marcia K. Johnson,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article An Age-Related Deficit in Prefrontal Cortical Function Associated With Refreshing Information , 2022 .

[34]  Jens Schwarzbach,et al.  Cerebral Cortex Advance Access published May 27, 2004 Control of Object-based Attention in Human Cortex , 2022 .

[35]  S. Courtney,et al.  Attention and cognitive control as emergent properties of information representation in working memory , 2004, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[36]  Marcel Brass,et al.  Selection for Cognitive Control: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study on the Selection of Task-Relevant Information , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Prefrontal cortex and flexible cognitive control: rules without symbols. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[38]  Edward K Vogel,et al.  Fractionating Working Memory , 2005, Psychological science.

[39]  Andrew B. Leber,et al.  Coordination of Voluntary and Stimulus-Driven Attentional Control in Human Cortex , 2005, Psychological science.

[40]  B. Douglas Ward,et al.  Deconvolution Analysis of FMRI Time Series Data , 2006 .

[41]  J. Jonides,et al.  Brain mechanisms of proactive interference in working memory , 2006, Neuroscience.

[42]  S. Yantis,et al.  Selective visual attention and perceptual coherence , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[43]  Joseph B. Sala,et al.  Binding of What and Where During Working Memory Maintenance , 2007, Cortex.

[44]  M. D’Esposito Working memory. , 2008, Handbook of clinical neurology.