A negative association between video game experience and proactive cognitive control.

Some evidence demonstrates that video game experience has a beneficial effect on visuospatial cognition. In contrast, other evidence indicates that video game experience may be negatively related to cognitive control. In this study we examined the specificity of the influence of video game experience on cognitive control. Participants with high and low video game experience performed the Stroop task while event-related brain potentials were recorded. The behavioral data revealed no difference between high and low gamers for the Stroop interference effect and a reduction in the conflict adaptation effect in high gamers. The amplitude of the medial frontal negativity and a frontal slow wave was attenuated in high gamers, and there was no effect of gaming status on the conflict slow potential. These data lead to the suggestion that video game experience has a negative influence on proactive, but not reactive, cognitive control.

[1]  M. Bouvard,et al.  Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and video games: A comparative study of hyperactive and control children , 2008, European Psychiatry.

[2]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[3]  J. Funk,et al.  Video and computer games in the '90s: children's time commitment and game preference. , 1996, Children today.

[4]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Conflict and Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.

[5]  Mark J Lowe,et al.  Media Violence Exposure and Frontal Lobe Activation Measured by Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging in Aggressive and Nonaggressive Adolescents , 2005, Journal of computer assisted tomography.

[6]  J. Pratt,et al.  Playing an Action Video Game Reduces Gender Differences in Spatial Cognition , 2007, Psychological science.

[7]  B. Kopell,et al.  The Stroop effect: brain potentials localize the source of interference. , 1981, Science.

[8]  C. Anderson,et al.  Violent video games: The newest media violence hazard. , 2003 .

[9]  H. Mayberg,et al.  An ERP study of the temporal course of the Stroop color-word interference effect , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[11]  H. Huynh,et al.  Estimation of the Box Correction for Degrees of Freedom from Sample Data in Randomized Block and Split-Plot Designs , 1976 .

[12]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  Katherine E. Buckley,et al.  Violent Video Game Effects on Children and Adolescents: Theory, Research, and Public Policy , 2007 .

[14]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Conflict Monitoring and Adjustments in Control , 2004, Science.

[15]  R. West,et al.  Neural correlates of cognitive control and conflict detection in the Stroop and digit-location tasks , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  R. West,et al.  Tracking the temporal dynamics of updating cognitive control: an examination of error processing. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[17]  Christopher P. Barlett,et al.  Video Game Effects—Confirmed, Suspected, and Speculative , 2009 .

[18]  Nicholas Wymbs,et al.  Neural correlates of conflict processing , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  G. Baylis,et al.  Effects of increased response dominance and contextual disintegration on the Stroop interference effect in older adults. , 1998, Psychology and aging.

[20]  Edward Swing Attention abilities, media exposure, school performance, personality, and aggression , 2008 .

[21]  H. McNeely,et al.  Neurophysiological evidence for disturbances of conflict processing in patients with schizophrenia. , 2003, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[22]  Katherine E. Buckley,et al.  Violent video game effects on children and adolescents. A review of the literature. , 2005, Minerva pediatrica.

[23]  T. Braver,et al.  Cognitive control, goal maintenance, and prefrontal function in healthy aging. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[24]  R. C. Oldfield THE ASSESSMENT AND ANALYSIS OF HANDEDNESS , 1971 .

[25]  L. Jacoby,et al.  Strategy-dependent changes in memory: Effects on behavior and brain activity , 2003, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[26]  S. Pliszka,et al.  Attention‐Deficit‐Hyperactivity Disorder: An Update , 2009, Pharmacotherapy.

[27]  T. Braver,et al.  Explaining the many varieties of working memory variation: Dual mechanisms of cognitive control. , 2007 .

[28]  E. Donchin,et al.  Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[29]  Mark J Lowe,et al.  Media violence exposure and executive functioning in aggressive and control adolescents. , 2005, Journal of clinical psychology.

[30]  C. S. Green,et al.  The Cognitive Neuroscience of Video Games , 2004 .

[31]  D. Gentile,et al.  The impact of video games on training surgeons in the 21st century. , 2007, Archives of surgery.

[32]  R. West,et al.  Effects of task context and fluctuations of attention on neural activity supporting performance of the Stroop task , 2000, Brain Research.

[33]  Alan T. Pope,et al.  Extended Attention Span Training System: Video Game Neurotherapy for Attention Deficit Disorder. , 1996 .

[34]  Todd S. Braver,et al.  A model of dual control mechanisms through anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex interactions , 2006, Neurocomputing.

[35]  Hillary D. Schwarb,et al.  The influence of aging and frontal function on the neural correlates of regulative and evaluative aspects of cognitive control. , 2006, Neuropsychology.

[36]  C. S. Green,et al.  Action video game modifies visual selective attention , 2003, Nature.

[37]  Alan T. Pope,et al.  Extended attention span training system , 1991 .

[38]  J. Stroop Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .