The Role of Dopamine in Motor Flexibility

Humans carry out many daily tasks in a seemingly automatic fashion. However, when unexpected changes in the environment occur, we have the capacity to inhibit prepotent behavior and replace it with an alternative one. Such behavioral flexibility is a hallmark of executive functions. The neurotransmitter dopamine is known to be crucial for fast, efficient, and accurate cognitive flexibility. Despite the perceived similarities between cognitive and motor flexibility, less is known regarding the role of dopamine within the motor domain. Therefore, the aim of this study was to determine the role of dopamine in motor flexibility. In a double-blind, five-session, within-subject pharmacological experiment, human participants performed an RT task within a probabilistic context that was either predictable or unpredictable. The probabilistic nature of the predictable context resulted in prediction errors. This required participants to replace the prepotent or prepared action with an unprepared action (motor flexibility). The task was overlearned, and changes in context were explicitly instructed, thus controlling for contributions from other dopamine-related processes such as probabilistic or reversal learning and interactions with other types of uncertainty. We found that dopamine receptor blockade by high-dose haloperidol (D1/D2 dopamine receptors) impaired participants' ability to react to unexpected events occurring in a predictable context, which elicit large prediction errors and necessitate motor flexibility. This effect was not observed with selective D2 receptor blockade (sulpiride), with a general increase in tonic dopamine levels (levodopa), or during an unpredictable context, which evoked minimal prediction error. We propose that dopamine is vital in responding to low-level prediction errors about stimulus outcome that requires motor flexibility.

[1]  B. Franke,et al.  Dissociable Effects of Dopamine and Serotonin on Reversal Learning , 2013, Neuron.

[2]  Marjan Jahanshahi,et al.  The role of the basal ganglia and its cortical connections in sequence learning: Evidence from implicit and explicit sequence learning in Parkinson's disease , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  J. Rothwell,et al.  Action Reprogramming in Parkinson's Disease: Response to Prediction Error Is Modulated by Levels of Dopamine , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  Sven Bestmann,et al.  Punishment-Induced Behavioral and Neurophysiological Variability Reveals Dopamine-Dependent Selection of Kinematic Movement Parameters , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  Angela J. Yu,et al.  Uncertainty, Neuromodulation, and Attention , 2005, Neuron.

[6]  D. G. Woolley,et al.  Asymmetric interlimb transfer of concurrent adaptation to opposing dynamic forces , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  S. Floresco Prefrontal dopamine and behavioral flexibility: shifting from an “inverted-U” toward a family of functions , 2013, Front. Neurosci..

[8]  F. Bymaster,et al.  The in vivo effects of olanzapine and other antipsychotic agents on receptor occupancy and antagonism of dopamine D1, D2, D3, 5HT2A and muscarinic receptors , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[9]  P. Redgrave,et al.  Is the short-latency dopamine response too short to signal reward error? , 1999, Trends in Neurosciences.

[10]  D. Rubin,et al.  Estimation in Covariance Components Models , 1981 .

[11]  R. Dolan,et al.  Dopamine, Time, and Impulsivity in Humans , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Encoding uncertainty in the hippocampus , 2006, Neural Networks.

[13]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Impulsive Personality Predicts Dopamine-Dependent Changes in Frontostriatal Activity during Component Processes of Working Memory , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  S. O'Connor,et al.  The pharmacology of sulpiride--a dopamine receptor antagonist. , 1982, General pharmacology.

[15]  M. Ragozzino,et al.  The effects of dopamine D(1) receptor blockade in the prelimbic-infralimbic areas on behavioral flexibility. , 2002, Learning & memory.

[16]  B. Franke,et al.  Human cognitive flexibility depends on dopamine D2 receptor signaling , 2011, Psychopharmacology.

[17]  D. Durstewitz,et al.  The Dual-State Theory of Prefrontal Cortex Dopamine Function with Relevance to Catechol-O-Methyltransferase Genotypes and Schizophrenia , 2008, Biological Psychiatry.

[18]  W. Beatty,et al.  Problem Solving in Parkinson's Disease: Comparison of Performance on the Wisconsin and California Card Sorting Tests , 1990, Journal of geriatric psychiatry and neurology.

[19]  D. Linszen,et al.  Occupancy of dopamine D2 receptors by antipsychotic drugs is related to nicotine addiction in young patients with schizophrenia , 2005, Psychopharmacology.

[20]  A. Björklund,et al.  Role of serotonin neurons in the induction of levodopa‐ and graft‐induced dyskinesias in Parkinson's disease , 2010, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[21]  B. Averbeck,et al.  Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience Systems Neuroscience Materials and Methods , 2022 .

[22]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Dynamic Dopamine Modulation in the Basal Ganglia: A Neurocomputational Account of Cognitive Deficits in Medicated and Nonmedicated Parkinsonism , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[23]  C. Montag,et al.  Frontostriatal Involvement in Task Switching Depends on Genetic Differences in D2 Receptor Density , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  Peter Dayan,et al.  A Neural Substrate of Prediction and Reward , 1997, Science.

[25]  Patrick W Stroman,et al.  Role of the basal ganglia in switching a planned response , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.

[26]  R. Cools,et al.  Establishing the dopamine dependency of human striatal signals during reward and punishment reversal learning. , 2014, Cerebral cortex.

[27]  T. Robbins,et al.  Cognitive inflexibility after prefrontal serotonin depletion is behaviorally and neurochemically specific. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[28]  P. Dayan,et al.  Tonic dopamine: opportunity costs and the control of response vigor , 2007, Psychopharmacology.

[29]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  A mechanistic account of striatal dopamine function in human cognition: psychopharmacological studies with cabergoline and haloperidol. , 2006, Behavioral neuroscience.

[30]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Perceptual Learning, Motor Learning and Automaticity Switching from Automatic to Controlled Behavior: Cortico-basal Ganglia Mechanisms , 2022 .

[31]  T. Robbins,et al.  Mechanisms of cognitive set flexibility in Parkinson's disease. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[32]  C. Mathys,et al.  Hierarchical Prediction Errors in Midbrain and Basal Forebrain during Sensory Learning , 2013, Neuron.

[33]  C. Gerfen The neostriatal mosaic: multiple levels of compartmental organization in the basal ganglia. , 1992, Annual review of neuroscience.

[34]  Raymond J. Dolan,et al.  Information theory, novelty and hippocampal responses: unpredicted or unpredictable? , 2005, Neural Networks.

[35]  S. Kapur,et al.  Predicting haloperidol occupancy of central dopamine D2 receptors from plasma levels , 2000, Psychopharmacology.

[36]  William D. Penny,et al.  The Role of Dopamine in Temporal Uncertainty , 2016, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[37]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Genetic triple dissociation reveals multiple roles for dopamine in reinforcement learning , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[38]  H. Meltzer,et al.  Effects of antipsychotic drugs on extracellular dopamine levels in rat medial prefrontal cortex and nucleus accumbens. , 1999, The Journal of pharmacology and experimental therapeutics.

[39]  J. Mink THE BASAL GANGLIA: FOCUSED SELECTION AND INHIBITION OF COMPETING MOTOR PROGRAMS , 1996, Progress in Neurobiology.

[40]  R. Dolan,et al.  Dopamine-dependent prediction errors underpin reward-seeking behaviour in humans , 2006, Nature.

[41]  A. Grace Phasic versus tonic dopamine release and the modulation of dopamine system responsivity: A hypothesis for the etiology of schizophrenia , 1991, Neuroscience.

[42]  Peter Dayan,et al.  Acetylcholine in cortical inference , 2002, Neural Networks.

[43]  C. Fiebach,et al.  Dissociable fronto-striatal effects of dopamine D2 receptor stimulation on cognitive versus motor flexibility , 2013, Cortex.

[44]  Raymond J. Dolan,et al.  Dopamine, Affordance and Active Inference , 2012, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[45]  M. Frank,et al.  Striatal Dopamine Predicts Outcome-Specific Reversal Learning and Its Sensitivity to Dopaminergic Drug Administration , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[46]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Bayesian model selection for group studies , 2009, NeuroImage.

[47]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Influence of Uncertainty and Surprise on Human Corticospinal Excitability during Preparation for Action , 2008, Current Biology.

[48]  Walter Paulus,et al.  Dopaminergic modulation of long‐lasting direct current‐induced cortical excitability changes in the human motor cortex , 2006 .

[49]  Bogdan Draganski,et al.  Dopamine reverses reward insensitivity in apathy following globus pallidus lesions , 2013, Cortex.

[50]  Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al.  Learning the value of information in an uncertain world , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[51]  P. Redgrave,et al.  The basal ganglia: a vertebrate solution to the selection problem? , 1999, Neuroscience.

[52]  Okihide Hikosaka,et al.  Cortico‐basal ganglia mechanisms for overcoming innate, habitual and motivational behaviors , 2011, The European journal of neuroscience.

[53]  M. Reimold,et al.  Occupancy of dopamine D1, D2 and serotonin2A receptors in schizophrenic patients treated with flupentixol in comparison with risperidone and haloperidol , 2006, Psychopharmacology.

[54]  A. Björklund,et al.  Serotonin Neuron Loss and Nonmotor Symptoms Continue in Parkinson’s Patients Treated with Dopamine Grafts , 2012, Science Translational Medicine.

[55]  B. Franke,et al.  Dissociable Effects of Dopamine and Serotonin on Reversal Learning , 2013, Neuron.

[56]  J. Neill,et al.  H1-histamine Receptor Affinity Predicts Short-term Weight Gain for Typical and Atypical Antipsychotic Drugs , 2003, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[57]  M. Horstink,et al.  Cognitive and motor shifting aptitude disorder in Parkinson's disease. , 1984, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[58]  Ethan R. Buch,et al.  Cortical and subcortical interactions during action reprogramming and their related white matter pathways , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[59]  D. Wong,et al.  Olanzapine: a basic science update , 1999, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[60]  Etienne Olivier,et al.  Short-Latency Influence of Medial Frontal Cortex on Primary Motor Cortex during Action Selection under Conflict , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[61]  T. Robbins,et al.  Cognitive Inflexibility After Prefrontal Serotonin Depletion , 2004, Science.

[62]  S. Floresco,et al.  Ventral Striatal Dopamine Modulation of Different Forms of Behavioral Flexibility , 2009, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[63]  H. Bergman,et al.  Goal-directed and habitual control in the basal ganglia: implications for Parkinson's disease , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[64]  T. Robbins,et al.  Enhanced or impaired cognitive function in Parkinson's disease as a function of dopaminergic medication and task demands. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[65]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.10 No.7 July 2006 Special Issue: Probabilistic models of cognition Bayesian decision theory in sensorimotor control , 2022 .

[66]  S. Floresco,et al.  Multiple Dopamine Receptor Subtypes in the Medial Prefrontal Cortex of the Rat Regulate Set-Shifting , 2006, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[67]  H. Meltzer,et al.  ACP-103, a 5-HT2A/2C inverse agonist, potentiates haloperidol-induced dopamine release in rat medial prefrontal cortex and nucleus accumbens , 2005, Psychopharmacology.

[68]  Joshua L. Plotkin,et al.  Synaptically driven state transitions in distal dendrites of striatal spiny neurons , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[69]  E. Wagenmakers,et al.  Testing theories of post-error slowing , 2011, Attention, perception & psychophysics.