Information Relevance, Working Memory, and the Consideration of Alternatives

People routinely focus on one hypothesis and avoid consideration of alternative hypotheses on problems requiring decisions between possible states of the world–-for example, on the “pseudodiagnosticity” task (Doherty, Mynatt, Tweney, & Schiavo, 1979). In order to account for behaviour on such “inference” problems, it is proposed that people can hold in working memory, and operate upon, but one alternative at a time, and that they have a bias to test the hypothesis they think true. In addition to being an ex post facto explanation of data selection in inference tasks, this conceptualization predicts that there are situations in which people will consider alternatives. These are: 1. “action” problems, where the alternatives are possible courses of action; 2. “inference” problems, in which evidence favours an alternative hypothesis. Experiment 1 tested the first prediction. Subjects were given action or inference problems, each with two alternatives and two items of data relevant to each alternative. They received probabilistic information about the relation between one datum and one alternative and picked one value from among the other three possible pairs of such relations. Two findings emerged; (1) a strong tendency to select information about only one alternative with inferences; and (2) a strong tendency, compared to inferences, to select information about both alternatives with actions. Experiment 2 tested the second prediction. It was predicted that data suggesting that one alternative was incorrect would lead many subjects to consider, and select information about, the other alternative. For actions, it was predicted that this manipulation would have no effect. Again the data were as predicted.

[1]  Reid Hastie,et al.  Revision of beliefs when a hypothesis is eliminated from consideration. , 1985 .

[2]  Yaacov Trope,et al.  Information-gathering strategies in hypothesis-testing , 1983 .

[3]  Y. Kareev,et al.  Evaluating Negative Tests and Refutations in a Rule Discovery Task , 1993 .

[4]  Clifford R. Mynatt,et al.  The influence of feedback and diagnostic data on pseudodiagnosticity , 1981 .

[5]  Jonathan S. Evans,et al.  Bias in human reasoning , 1990 .

[6]  A. Tversky,et al.  Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases , 1974, Science.

[7]  W. James,et al.  The Principles of Psychology. , 1983 .

[8]  Herbert A. Simon,et al.  The Sciences of the Artificial , 1970 .

[9]  M E Doherty,et al.  'Pseudodiagnosticity' in an idealized medical problem-solving environment. , 1982, Journal of medical education.

[10]  Clifford R. Mynatt,et al.  Consequences of Confirmation and Disconfirmation in a Simulated Research Environment , 1978 .

[11]  P. Johnson-Laird,et al.  Psychology of Reasoning: Structure and Content , 1972 .

[12]  L. Cohen Can human irrationality be experimentally demonstrated? , 1981, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[13]  P C Wason,et al.  Reasoning about a Rule , 1968, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[14]  R. Skov,et al.  Information-gathering processes: Diagnosticity, hypothesis-confirmatory strategies, and perceived hypothesis confirmation. , 1986 .

[15]  S. Coren,et al.  Attention : contemporary theory and analysis , 1972 .

[16]  G. A. Miller THE PSYCHOLOGICAL REVIEW THE MAGICAL NUMBER SEVEN, PLUS OR MINUS TWO: SOME LIMITS ON OUR CAPACITY FOR PROCESSING INFORMATION 1 , 1956 .

[17]  R. Hogarth,et al.  BEHAVIORAL DECISION THEORY: PROCESSES OF JUDGMENT AND CHOICE , 1981 .

[18]  P. Wason On the Failure to Eliminate Hypotheses in a Conceptual Task , 1960 .

[19]  Reid Hastie,et al.  Tracing the footsteps of Sherlock Holmes: Cognitive representations of hypothesis testing , 1990, Memory & cognition.

[20]  Y. Trope,et al.  Confirmatory and diagnosing strategies in social information gathering. , 1982 .

[21]  Baruch Fischhoff,et al.  Diagnosticity and pseudodiagnosticity. , 1983 .

[22]  Ryan D. Tweney,et al.  Strategies of Rule Discovery in an Inference Task , 1980 .

[23]  Jonathan Evans Heuristic and analytic processes in reasoning , 1984 .

[24]  Mark Snyder,et al.  Behavioral confirmation in social interaction: From social perception to social reality. , 1978 .

[25]  N. Cowan Activation, attention, and short-term memory , 1993, Memory & cognition.

[26]  K. A. Ericsson,et al.  Protocol Analysis: Verbal Reports as Data , 1984 .

[27]  N. Cowan Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, and their mutual constraints within the human information-processing system. , 1988, Psychological bulletin.

[28]  E. Brunswik Perception and the Representative Design of Psychological Experiments , 1957 .

[29]  W. Edwards,et al.  Decision Analysis and Behavioral Research , 1986 .

[30]  T. Pearson,et al.  Direct comparison of the efficacy of intuitive and analytical cognition in expert judgment , 1987, IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics.

[31]  Michael E. Doherty,et al.  Data selection in a minimal hypothesis testing task , 1991 .

[32]  J. Klayman,et al.  Hypothesis testing in rule discovery: Strategy, structure, and content. , 1989 .