Infants' sensitivity to pictorial depth cues: a review and meta-analysis of looking studies.

This paper reviews habituation-dishabituation and preferential-looking studies on the emergence of sensitivity to pictorial depth cues in infancy. This research can be subdivided into two groups. While one group of studies has established responsiveness to pictorial depth cues at 3-5 months of age, the other has found that the ability to extract pictorial 3D information emerges at about 6 months. In the former, young infants were tested for their ability to distinguish between displays that differ in spatial information provided by pictorial depth cues. The results of these studies might demonstrate that 3-5-month-old infants perceive spatial layout from pictorial cues. It is possible, however, that the infants in these studies responded primarily to low-level, two-dimensional stimulus differences. In contrast, the second group of studies controlled for the potential influence of lower-level stimulus features on the infants' experimental performance and more unambiguously demonstrated sensitivity to pictorial depth information in infants 6 months of age and older. In sum, the divergent findings of studies in this area may be resolved by assuming substantial developmental progress in infant sensitivity to pictorial depth cues during the first months of life.

[1]  Tomoko Imura,et al.  Infants’ sensitivity to shading and line junctions , 2008, Vision Research.

[2]  M. Kavšek Infant perception of static two-dimensional transparency information , 2009 .

[3]  Jacob Cohen Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[4]  Helmut Leder,et al.  The development of face processing , 2003 .

[5]  I. Sigel,et al.  HANDBOOK OF CHILD PSYCHOLOGY , 2006 .

[6]  Young infants’ sensitivity to shading stimuli with radial motion1 , 2005 .

[7]  A. Yonas,et al.  Infants’ perception of depth from cast shadows , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.

[8]  B. Bertenthal,et al.  The partial-lag design: A method for controlling spontaneous regression in the infant-control habituation paradigm* , 1983 .

[9]  R. Lécuyer,et al.  The use of perspective as a depth cue with a 2D display in 4 and 5-month-old infants. , 2007, Infant behavior & development.

[10]  M. Arterberry Infants' Sensitivity to the Depth Cue of Height-in-the-Picture-Plane , 2008 .

[11]  Infants' sensitivity to differential luminance as information about depth. , 1991, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[12]  R. Bhatt,et al.  Pictorial cues and three-dimensional information processing in early infancy. , 2001, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[13]  Y. Otsuka,et al.  Perceptual Transparency in 3- to 4-Month-Old Infants , 2006, Perception.

[14]  Ma Conway,et al.  Handbook of perception and cognition , 1996 .

[15]  Infants' perception of illusory contours in static and moving figures. , 2003, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[16]  J. Gibson The perception of the visual world , 1951 .

[17]  Guillaume A Rousselet,et al.  Inverting faces elicits sensitivity to race on the N170 component: a cross-cultural study. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[18]  C Owsley,et al.  The Role of Motion in Infants' Perception of Solid Shape , 1983, Perception.

[19]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Discrimination of Possible and Impossible Objects in Infancy , 2007, Psychological science.

[20]  E. Erdfelder,et al.  Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses , 2009, Behavior research methods.

[21]  J. Colombo,et al.  Visual pop-out in infants: Evidence for preattentive search in 3- and 4-month-olds , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[22]  Albert Yonas,et al.  Emergence of sensitivity to pictorial depth cues: Charting development in individual infants , 2002 .

[23]  P. Kellman Perception of three-dimensional form by human infants , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.

[24]  James E. Cutting,et al.  Chapter 3 – Perceiving Layout and Knowing Distances: The Integration, Relative Potency, and Contextual Use of Different Information about Depth* , 1995 .

[25]  Jazmine Orprecio,et al.  The eyes have it: visual pop-out in infants and adults. , 2006, Developmental science.

[26]  A. Yonas,et al.  Infants' responsiveness to pictorial depth cues in preferential-reaching studies: a meta-analysis. , 2009, Infant behavior & development.

[27]  M. Kavšek The Influence of Context on Amodal Completion in 5- and 7-Month-Old Infants , 2004 .

[28]  M. Kavšek The perception of static subjective contours in infancy. , 2002, Child development.

[29]  A Yonas,et al.  Infants Perceive Spatial Structure Specified by Line Junctions , 1994, Perception.

[30]  M. Kavšek Infants' perception of directional alignment of texture elements on a spherical surface , 2003 .

[31]  A. Yonas,et al.  Infant's ability to form a common representation of an object's shape from different pictorial depth cues: A transfer-across-cues study. , 2009, Infant behavior & development.

[32]  P. Kellman,et al.  Development of three-dimensional form perception. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[33]  M. Arterberry,et al.  Infant Visual Perception , 2007 .

[34]  R. Kaufmann-Hayoz,et al.  Development of infants' sensitivity for slow and rapid motions , 1985 .

[35]  Edgar Erdfelder,et al.  G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences , 2007, Behavior research methods.

[36]  Yumiko Otsuka,et al.  Development of Modal and Amodal Completion in Infants , 2006, Perception.

[37]  Albert Yonas,et al.  Infants' responsiveness to static-monocular depth information : a recovery from habituation approach , 1991 .

[38]  T. Meese,et al.  Performance data indicate summation for pictorial depth-cues in slanted surfaces. , 2004, Spatial vision.

[39]  Albert Yonas,et al.  Infants' sensitivity to familiar size as information for distance. , 1982 .

[40]  A. Yonas,et al.  Infants’ sensitivity to the depth cue of shading , 1985, Perception & psychophysics.

[41]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  On the perception of shape from shading , 1988, Nature.

[42]  Sherryse L. Corrow,et al.  The development of the ability of infants to utilize static cues to create and access representations of object shape. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[43]  A. Yonas,et al.  Infant Perception of Surface Texture and Relative Height as Distance Information: A Preferential-Reaching Study. , 2010, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[44]  Asymmetry in the detection of shapes from shading in infants1 , 2008 .

[45]  R. Fox,et al.  The Ames window illusion: perception of illusory motion by human infants. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[46]  R. Bhatt,et al.  Perception of three-dimensional cues in early infancy. , 1998, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[47]  R. Lécuyer,et al.  Representation of the third dimension: The use of perspective cues by 3- and 4-month-old infants , 2003 .

[48]  M. Kavšek Infant perception of object unity in static displays , 2004 .

[49]  R. Bhatt,et al.  Three-month-olds' sensitivity to orientation cues in the three-dimensional depth plane. , 2006, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[50]  Marc H. Bornstein,et al.  Mechanisms of Visual Object Recognition in Infancy: Five-Month-Olds Generalize Beyond the Interpolation of Familiar Views. , 2007, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[51]  S. Shuwairi,et al.  Preference for impossible figures in 4-month-olds. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[52]  M. Kavšek Infants' responsiveness to line junctions in curved objects. , 1999, Journal of experimental child psychology.