A Form of Ideational Apraxia as a Delective Deficit of Contention Scheduling

In this paper we studied three brain-damaged patients: the first two, DR and FG, had limb apraxia whilst the third was a control patient (WH2) with an executive function disorder but without limb apraxia. DR and FG were impaired in carrying out everyday actions, whilst they maintained the ability to sequence photographs representing those same activities. The failure in the action production task was not caused by visual agnosia for objects, as the patients could recognise them from sight. Nor was it produced by a loss of knowledge about their functions (De Renzi & Lucchelli, 1988), as DR and FG could identify objects from descriptions of their use. WH2's pattern of performance doubly dissociated from that of the apraxic patients, namely spared action production on the multiple object test, but faulty sequencing of photographs. WH2's difficulties in sequencing photographs were not due to a failure to understand the task, as she could sequence stimuli other than actions (e.g., shapes and numbers). Nor were the differences due to a loss of knowledge about the actions, since she could perform and identify them from photographs. These results show that the kind of apraxia observed in DR and FG is not produced by a degraded action sequence representation (Lehmkuhl & Poeck, 1981; Poeck & Lehmkuhl, 1980). We interpreted our results within a contention scheduling model (Cooper & Shallice, 2000; Norman & Shallice, 1986).

[1]  H. Nelson A Modified Card Sorting Test Sensitive to Frontal Lobe Defects , 1976, Cortex.

[2]  G. Humphreys,et al.  The Role of Semantic Knowledge and Working Memory in Everyday Tasks , 2000, Brain and Cognition.

[3]  Eric A. Roy,et al.  Neuropsychological studies of apraxia and related disorders , 1985 .

[4]  Michael W. Montgomery,et al.  Analysis of a disorder of everyday action , 1995 .

[5]  Stéphane Lehéricy,et al.  Distinct prefrontal activations in processing sequence at the sentence and script level: An fMRI study , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  Michael W. Montgomery,et al.  Naturalistic action impairment in closed head injury. , 1998, Neuropsychology.

[7]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Grasping objects and grasping action meanings: the dual role of monkey rostroventral premotor cortex (area F5). , 1998, Novartis Foundation symposium.

[8]  L. Buxbaum,et al.  The Role of Semantic Memory in Object Use , 1997 .

[9]  E. Weigl,et al.  On the psychology of so-called processes of abstraction. , 1941 .

[10]  Michael W. Montgomery,et al.  The quantitative description of action disorganisation after brain damage: a case study , 1991 .

[11]  D. Perrett,et al.  Integration of form and motion in the anterior superior temporal polysensory area (STPa) of the macaque monkey. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  D. Norman Categorization of action slips. , 1981 .

[13]  D. Norman,et al.  Attention to action: Willed and automatic control , 1980 .

[14]  E. Renzi,et al.  Ideational apraxia: A quantitative study , 1968 .

[15]  Miceli Gabriele,et al.  Batteria per l'analisi dei deficit afasici. B.A.D.A. , 1994 .

[16]  K. Heilman,et al.  A Cognitive Neuropsychological Model of Limb Praxis , 1991 .

[17]  E. Roy,et al.  Common Considerations In The Study of Limb, Verbal And Oral Apraxia , 1985 .

[18]  A. Luria Higher Cortical Functions in Man , 1980, Springer US.

[19]  T. Shallice,et al.  CONTENTION SCHEDULING AND THE CONTROL OF ROUTINE ACTIVITIES , 2000, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[20]  E. Renzi,et al.  Oral Apraxia and Aphasia , 1966 .

[21]  Paolo Nichelli,et al.  Verbal and Non-Verbal Short-Term Memory Impairment Following Hemispheric Damage , 1975, Cortex.

[22]  Glyn W Humphreys,et al.  AGNOSIA WITHOUT PROSOPAGNOSIA OR ALEXIA: EVIDENCE FOR STORED VISUAL MEMORIES SPECIFIC TO OBJECTS. , 1998, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[23]  K. Poeck,et al.  A Disturbance in the Conceptual Organization of Actions in Patients with Ideational Apraxia , 1981, Cortex.

[24]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  BORB: Birmingham Object Recognition Battery , 2017 .

[25]  A. Sirigu,et al.  Distinct Frontal Regions for Processing Sentence Syntax and Story Grammar , 1998, Cortex.

[26]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  Selective Impairments in Managerial Knowledge Following Pre-Frontal Cortex Damage , 1995, Cortex.

[27]  L. Buxbaum,et al.  Ideational apraxia and naturalistic action. , 1998, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[28]  E. Renzi,et al.  Methods of Limb Apraxia Examination and Their Bearing On The Interpretation of The Disorder , 1985 .

[29]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Disordered action schema and action disorganisation syndrome , 1998 .

[30]  Roger C. Schank,et al.  SCRIPTS, PLANS, GOALS, AND UNDERSTANDING , 1988 .

[31]  E. Renzi,et al.  Imitating gestures. A quantitative approach to ideomotor apraxia. , 1980, Archives of neurology.

[32]  D. Norman,et al.  Attention to Action: Willed and Automatic Control of Behavior Technical Report No. 8006. , 1980 .

[33]  K. Poeck Ideational apraxia , 2004, Journal of Neurology.

[34]  K. Poeck,et al.  Ideatory Apraxia in a Left-Handed Patient with Right-Sided Brain Lesion , 1980, Cortex.

[35]  K. Heilman,et al.  Ideational apraxia: A deficit in tool selection and use , 1989, Annals of neurology.

[36]  David L. Shapiro,et al.  On the Psychology of , 1996 .