Choosing versus Rejecting: The Impact of Goal-Task Compatibility on Decision Confidence

This research posits that goal orientation influences the impact of the type of decision task (selection vs. rejection) on decision confidence. The data reported in a series of three experiments show that promotion-focused individuals tend to be more confident in their decisions in the context of a selection rather than a rejection task, whereas for prevention-focused individuals this effect is reversed. This research suggests that goal-task compatibility underlies the observed effects and shows that the impact of goal orientation can transfer the value of the process onto the judgment of the outcomes, as suggested by prior research, and have a significant impact on individuals’ decision confidence. The empirical data support these propositions across different decision contexts, offering new insights into the role of goal-task compatibility in individual decision processes. Individuals frequently are confronted with two types of decisions: decisions in which they must select an alternative from a given set, and decisions in which they must give up one or more of the available alternatives. To illustrate, an individual might consider buying an item (selection decision) and later consider returning the item (rejection decision). Although choosing one of two options is normatively equivalent to rejecting the other option, it has been shown that there are systematic differences in how individuals decide to select and to give up choice alternatives. Prior research has shown that evaluations of a given object are asymmetric depending on whether the object is being acquired or forfeited, such that individuals are generally willing to pay less for a given product than they are willing to accept for giving up the same product (Kahneman, Knetsch, & Thaler, 1991; Tversky & Kahneman, 1991). It has further been demonstrated that more extreme options (e.g., options having more positive and more negative dimensions) have a higher probability of being chosen as well as rejected relative to less extreme options (Dunning & Parpal, 1989; Shafir, 1993, see also Dhar & Wertenbroch, 2000). Prior research has also examined the impact of the nature of the decision task on indi

[1]  E. Higgins,et al.  Transfer of value from fit. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[2]  I. Simonson,et al.  Choice Based on Reasons: The Case of Attraction and Compromise Effects , 1989 .

[3]  J. Baron,et al.  Omission and commission in judgment and choice , 1991 .

[4]  Alexander Chernev,et al.  The Impact of Common Features on Consumer Preferences: A Case of Confirmatory Reasoning , 2001 .

[5]  Leif D. Nelson,et al.  Intuitive confidence: choosing between intuitive and nonintuitive alternatives. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[6]  E. Higgins Promotion and Prevention: Regulatory Focus as A Motivational Principle , 1998 .

[7]  Alexander Chernev,et al.  Goal Orientation and Consumer Preference for the Status Quo , 2004 .

[8]  Alexander Chernev,et al.  Articulation Compatibility in Eliciting Price Bids , 2006 .

[9]  E. Higgins,et al.  To "do the right thing" or to "just do it": locomotion and assessment as distinct self-regulatory imperatives. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[10]  Robert D. Hoge,et al.  Confidence in decision as an index of perceived accuracy of information processing , 1970 .

[11]  A. Tversky,et al.  Contingent weighting in judgment and choice , 1988 .

[12]  E. Tory Higgins,et al.  Locomotion, Assessment, and Regulatory Fit: Value Transfer from 'How' to 'What' , 2003 .

[13]  A. Tversky,et al.  Loss Aversion in Riskless Choice: A Reference-Dependent Model , 1991 .

[14]  Eldar Shafir,et al.  Choosing versus rejecting: Why some options are both better and worse than others , 1993, Memory & cognition.

[15]  I. Simonson,et al.  Attribute–Task Compatibility as a Determinant of Consumer Preference Reversals: , 1997 .

[16]  C. Whan Park,et al.  Choosing What I Want versus Rejecting What I Do Not Want: An Application of Decision Framing to Product Option Choice Decisions , 2000 .

[17]  G. W. Fischer,et al.  Strategy compatibility, scale compatibility, and the prominence effect. , 1993 .

[18]  J. Baron,et al.  Outcome Knowledge, Regret, and Omission Bias , 1995 .

[19]  Angela Y. Lee,et al.  Bringing the frame into focus: the influence of regulatory fit on processing fluency and persuasion. , 2004, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[20]  A. Tversky,et al.  Choice in Context: Tradeoff Contrast and Extremeness Aversion , 1992 .

[21]  M. Gill,et al.  On the genesis of confidence. , 1998 .

[22]  E. Higgins,et al.  Ideal versus ought predilections for approach and avoidance: distinct self-regulatory systems. , 1994, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[23]  N Liberman,et al.  Promotion and prevention choices between stability and change. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[24]  Irwin P. Levin,et al.  Choosing versus rejecting options at different stages of decision making , 1998 .

[25]  J. Landman Regret and Elation Following Action and Inaction , 1987 .

[26]  R. Dhar,et al.  Consumer Choice between Hedonic and Utilitarian Goods , 2000 .

[27]  R. Dhar Consumer Preference for a No-Choice Option , 1997 .

[28]  Gordon F. Pitz,et al.  Confidence and decision speed in the revision of opinion , 1968 .

[29]  E. Higgins,et al.  Regulatory Focus and Strategic Inclinations : Promotion and Prevention in Decision-Making , 1997 .

[30]  Jack B. Soll,et al.  Overconfidence: It Depends on How, What, and Whom You Ask. , 1999, Organizational behavior and human decision processes.

[31]  E. Higgins,et al.  Regulatory fit and persuasion: transfer from "Feeling Right.". , 2004, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[32]  Jonathan Baron,et al.  Reference Points and Omission Bias , 1994 .

[33]  E. Higgins Making a good decision: value from fit. , 2000, The American psychologist.

[34]  R. Dhar,et al.  The Effect of Forced Choice on Choice , 2003 .

[35]  A. Tversky Elimination by aspects: A theory of choice. , 1972 .

[36]  Maurice E. Schweitzer,et al.  DISENTANGLING STATUS QUO AND OMISSION EFFECTS: AN EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS , 1994 .

[37]  Stuart J. Russell,et al.  Do the right thing , 1991 .

[38]  Daniel Kahneman,et al.  Anomalies: The Endowment Effect, Loss Aversion, and Status Quo Bias , 1991 .

[39]  E. Higgins,et al.  Enjoying Goal-Directed Action: The Role of Regulatory Fit , 2002, Psychological science.

[40]  Dan Ariely,et al.  Goal-Based Construction of Preferences: Task Goals and the Prominence Effect , 1999 .

[41]  D. Dunning,et al.  Mental addition versus subtraction in counterfactual reasoning: on assessing the impact of personal actions and life events. , 1989, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[42]  Alexander Chernev,et al.  Extremeness Aversion and Attribute-Balance Effects in Choice , 2004 .

[43]  A. Chernev Goal-Attribute Compatibility in Consumer Choice , 2004 .