A Two-Point Visual Control Model of Steering

When steering down a winding road, drivers have been shown to use both near and far regions of the road for guidance during steering. We propose a model of steering that explicitly embodies this idea, using both a ‘near point’ to maintain a central lane position and a ‘far point’ to account for the upcoming roadway. Unlike control models that integrate near and far information to compute curvature or more complex features, our model relies solely on one perceptually plausible feature of the near and far points, namely the visual direction to each point. The resulting parsimonious model can be run in simulation within a realistic highway environment to facilitate direct comparison between model and human behavior. Using such simulations, we demonstrate that the proposed two-point model is able to account for four interesting aspects of steering behavior: curve negotiation with occluded visual regions, corrective steering after a lateral drift, lane changing, and individual differences.

[1]  David N. Lee,et al.  Where we look when we steer , 1994, Nature.

[2]  Erwin R. Boer Tangent point oriented curve negotiation , 1996, Proceedings of Conference on Intelligent Vehicles.

[3]  B. Rogers,et al.  Going against the flow , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[4]  John P. Wann,et al.  How do we control high speed steering , 2004 .

[5]  David H. Weir,et al.  New Results in Driver Steering Control Models , 1977 .

[6]  V. Virsu Underestimation of curvature and task dependence in visual perception of form , 1971 .

[7]  J. Wann,et al.  Heading perception and the allocation of attention , 2000, Vision Research.

[8]  Richard Wilkie,et al.  Controlling steering and judging heading: retinal flow, visual direction, and extraretinal information. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  Guy Wallis,et al.  An Unexpected Role for Visual Feedback in Vehicle Steering Control , 2002, Current Biology.

[10]  J. Groeger Understanding Driving: Applying Cognitive Psychology to a Complex Everyday Task , 2000 .

[11]  H Godthelp,et al.  Vehicle Control During Curve Driving , 1986, Human factors.

[12]  Dario D Salvucci,et al.  An egocentric account of the visual guidance of locomotion , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[13]  D Regan,et al.  How do we avoid confounding the direction we are looking and the direction we are moving? , 1982, Science.

[14]  J. Mathur Protein conditioners for split ends , 2000, Current Biology.

[15]  R Gray,et al.  Attentional modulation of motion-in-depth processing , 2000, Vision Research.

[16]  Julie M. Harris,et al.  Guidance of locomotion on foot uses perceived target location rather than optic flow , 1998, Current Biology.

[17]  Dario D. Salvucci Predicting the effects of in-car interface use on driver performance: an integrated model approach , 2001, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[18]  C. Lebiere,et al.  The Atomic Components of Thought , 1998 .

[19]  Ichiro Masaki Vision-based mobile robots on highways , 1994, Adv. Robotics.

[20]  James A. Crowell,et al.  Ideal observer for heading judgments , 1996, Vision Research.

[21]  Richard M Wilkie,et al.  Eye-movements aid the control of locomotion. , 2003, Journal of vision.

[22]  D Shinar CURVE PERCEPTION AND ACCIDENTS ON CURVES: AN ILLUSIVE CURVE PHENOMENON? , 1977 .

[23]  N L Biggs,et al.  Directional guidance of motor vehicles--a preliminary survey and analysis. , 1966, Ergonomics.

[24]  J. Gibson Visually controlled locomotion and visual orientation in animals. , 1998, British journal of psychology.

[25]  K. R. Llewellyn,et al.  Visual guidance of locomotion. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.

[26]  Michael Land,et al.  Which parts of the road guide steering? , 1995, Nature.

[27]  D. Regan,et al.  Vernier step acuity and bisection acuity for texture-defined form , 1997, Vision Research.

[28]  M. Teghtsoonian,et al.  Scaling apparent distance in a natural outdoor setting , 1970 .

[29]  Robert S. Allison,et al.  Egocentric Direction and the Visual Guidance of Robot Locomotion Background, Theory and Implementation , 2002, Biologically Motivated Computer Vision.

[30]  Erwin R. Boer,et al.  Toward an Integrated Model of Driver Behavior in Cognitive Architecture , 2001 .

[31]  Brett R Fajen,et al.  Behavioral dynamics of steering, obstacle avoidance, and route selection. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[32]  James A. Crowell,et al.  Estimating heading during eye movements , 1994, Vision Research.

[33]  L. DeFelice Going against the flow , 2004, Nature.

[34]  W. V. Winsum,et al.  Lane change manoeuvres and safety margins , 1999 .

[35]  D E Kieras,et al.  A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: Part 1. Basic mechanisms. , 1997, Psychological review.

[36]  Dario D. Salvucci,et al.  The time course of a lane change: Driver control and eye-movement behavior , 2002 .

[37]  C. Koch,et al.  Is perception discrete or continuous? , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[38]  Constance S. Royden,et al.  From vision to action: experiments and models of steering control during driving. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[39]  B. Fajen Steering toward a goal by equalizing taus. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[40]  B. Fildes,et al.  The on effect of changes in curve geometry magnitude estimates of road-like perspective curvature , 1985 .

[41]  Richard Reviewer-Granger Unified Theories of Cognition , 1991, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[42]  Edmund Donges,et al.  A Two-Level Model of Driver Steering Behavior , 1978 .