The effect of food and water deprivation and satiation on recognition.

Food and water deprived and satiated subjects, as well as control subjects, were shown words presented tachistoscopically for .01 sec until word recognition. Five food-relevant, five water-relevant, and five neutral (animal) words of high string frequency were matched for letter confusability and letter predictability. Analyses of the data, in terms of number of presentations until recognition as well as number of words recognized at selected presentations, revealed that the amount but not the type of deprivation significantly altered word recognition. Moreover, the effect of motivation was significant already on the first slide presentation, while the effects of word characteristics (word category and generated value) occurred only after a number of presentations.

[1]  J. Spence Contribution of response bias to recognition thresholds. , 1963, Journal of abnormal and social psychology.

[2]  K. Spence,et al.  CHAPTER 12 – The Motivational Components of Manifest Anxiety: Drive and Drive Stimuli1 , 1966 .

[3]  L. Wispé,et al.  Physiological need, word frequency, and visual duration thresholds. , 1953, Journal of experimental psychology.

[4]  D. Rumelhart,et al.  Process of recognizing tachistoscopically presented words. , 1974, Psychological review.

[5]  Clarity versus identifiability of repeatedly flashed patterns. , 1973, Journal of experimental psychology.

[6]  J. W. Atkinson,et al.  Achievement motive and test anxiety conceived as motive to approach success and motive to avoid failure. , 1960, Journal of abnormal and social psychology.

[7]  R. Klatzky Human Memory: Structures And Processes , 1975 .

[8]  Donald E. Broadbent,et al.  Decision and stress , 1971 .

[9]  S. Hepler,et al.  Selective information processing , 1977, Memory & cognition.

[10]  R. Brubaker Models for the perception of speech and visual form: Weiant Wathen-Dunn, ed.: Cambridge, Mass., The M.I.T. Press, I–X, 470 pages , 1968 .

[11]  H. Kucera,et al.  Computational analysis of present-day American English , 1967 .

[12]  E. Walker,et al.  Action decrement and its relation to learning. , 1958 .

[13]  G. Oden Fuzziness in semantic memory: Choosing exemplars of subjective categories , 1977, Memory & cognition.

[14]  Charles D. Spielberger,et al.  Anxiety and behavior , 1966 .

[15]  E. D. Ferguson Relationship between Ego-Involvement Instructions and Various Motivation and Performance Measures , 1971 .

[16]  R. Haber,et al.  Two types of short-term visual storage , 1969 .

[17]  S. Glucksberg,et al.  The influence of strength of drive on functional fixedness and perceptual recognition. , 1962, Journal of experimental psychology.

[18]  G. Mandler,et al.  A study of anxiety and learning. , 1952, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[19]  B. Underwood,et al.  Meaningfulness and verbal learning , 1960 .

[20]  M. Eysenck,et al.  Effects of noise, activation level, and response dominance on retrieval from semantic memory. , 1975, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[21]  R. Atkinson,et al.  The control of short-term memory. , 1971, Scientific American.

[22]  R C JOHNSON,et al.  Word values, word frequency, and visual duration thresholds. , 1960, Psychological review.

[23]  E. D. Ferguson Effect of Ego-Involvement Instructions and Intralist Similarity on Stimulus Generalization Errors in Paired-Associate Learning , 1972, Psychological reports.

[24]  S. Garrod,et al.  An independence of mechanism in the origins of reading and classification-related semantic distance effects , 1977, Memory & cognition.

[25]  R. Margolin,et al.  Arousal effects in intentional recall and forgetting. , 1970, Journal of experimental psychology.