Title Electrophysiological evidence of heterogeneity in visual statistical learning in young children with ASD Permalink

Statistical learning is characterized by detection of regularities in one’s environment without an awareness or intention to learn, and it may play a critical role in language and social behavior. Accordingly, in this study we investigated the electrophysiological correlates of visual statistical learning in young children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) using an event-related potential shape learning paradigm, and we examined the relation between visual statistical learning and cognitive function. Compared to typically developing (TD) controls, the ASD group as a whole showed reduced evidence of learning as defined by N1 (early visual discrimination) and P300 (attention to novelty) components. Upon further analysis, in the ASD group there was a positive correlation between N1 amplitude difference and non-verbal IQ, and a positive correlation between P300 amplitude difference and adaptive social function. Children with ASD and a high non-verbal IQ and high adaptive social function demonstrated a distinctive pattern of learning. This is the first study to identify electrophysiological markers of visual statistical learning in children with ASD. Through this work we have demonstrated heterogeneity in statistical learning in ASD that maps onto non-verbal cognition and adaptive social function.

[1]  R. Lenroot,et al.  Heterogeneity within Autism Spectrum Disorders: What have We Learned from Neuroimaging Studies? , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[2]  I. Eigsti,et al.  Brief Report: A Comparison of Statistical Learning in School-Aged Children with High Functioning Autism and Typically Developing Peers , 2012, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[3]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Visual statistical learning in the newborn infant , 2011, Cognition.

[4]  Whitney Guthrie,et al.  Convergent validity of the Mullen Scales of Early Learning and the differential ability scales in children with autism spectrum disorders. , 2011, American journal on intellectual and developmental disabilities.

[5]  Daniel C. Richardson,et al.  Infants learn about objects from statistics and people. , 2011, Developmental psychology.

[6]  Joanne Arciuli,et al.  Statistical learning in typically developing children: the role of age and speed of stimulus presentation. , 2011, Developmental science.

[7]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  Timing is everything: Changes in presentation rate have opposite effects on auditory and visual implicit statistical learning , 2011, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[8]  Alexa R. Romberg,et al.  Statistical learning and language acquisition. , 2010, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.

[9]  Susan Y. Bookheimer,et al.  No Neural Evidence of Statistical Learning During Exposure to Artificial Languages in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders , 2010, Biological Psychiatry.

[10]  L. Klinger,et al.  Motor‐linked implicit learning in persons with autism spectrum disorders , 2010, Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research.

[11]  T. Striano,et al.  Eye contact and emotional face processing in 6-month-old infants: Advanced statistical methods applied to event-related potentials , 2010, Brain and Development.

[12]  Jessica F. Hay,et al.  Statistical learning in a natural language by 8-month-old infants. , 2009, Child development.

[13]  C. Nelson,et al.  Event Related Potentials in the Understanding of Autism Spectrum Disorders: An Analytical Review , 2009, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[14]  Michael C. Frank,et al.  Abstract Rule Learning for Visual Sequences in 8- and 11-Month-Olds. , 2009, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[15]  S. Mostofsky,et al.  Evidence that the pattern of visuomotor sequence learning is altered in children with autism , 2008, Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research.

[16]  William D Gaillard,et al.  Intact implicit learning of spatial context and temporal sequences in childhood autism spectrum disorder. , 2008, Neuropsychology.

[17]  John O. Willis,et al.  Mullen Scales of Early Learning: Ags Edition , 2008 .

[18]  B. Scholl,et al.  Multidimensional Visual Statistical Learning Visual Statistical Learning , 2005 .

[19]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Location, location, location: development of spatiotemporal sequence learning in infancy. , 2007, Child development.

[20]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Infant Rule Learning Facilitated by Speech , 2007, Psychological science.

[21]  Shauna M. Stark,et al.  Procedural Learning of a Visual Sequence in Individuals With Autism , 2007 .

[22]  Natacha Akshoomoff,et al.  Use of the Mullen Scales of Early Learning for the Assessment of Young Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders , 2006, Child neuropsychology : a journal on normal and abnormal development in childhood and adolescence.

[23]  C. Kasari,et al.  Joint attention and symbolic play in young children with autism: a randomized controlled intervention study. , 2006, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[24]  P. Perruchet,et al.  Implicit learning and statistical learning: one phenomenon, two approaches , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[25]  B. Scholl,et al.  The Automaticity of Visual Statistical Learning Statistical Learning , 2005 .

[26]  Scott Sinnett,et al.  Speech segmentation by statistical learning depends on attention , 2005, Cognition.

[27]  John E Richards,et al.  Familiarization, attention, and recognition memory in infancy: an event-related potential and cortical source localization study. , 2005, Developmental psychology.

[28]  J. Hampton,et al.  The prototype effect in recognition memory: intact in autism? , 2005, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[29]  D. Howard,et al.  Implicit sequence learning: effects of level of structure, adult age, and extended practice. , 2004, Psychology and aging.

[30]  Richard N Aslin,et al.  Statistical learning of new visual feature combinations by infants , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[31]  J. Saffran Constraints on Statistical Language Learning , 2002 .

[32]  R. Aslin,et al.  Statistical learning of higher-order temporal structure from visual shape sequences. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[33]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Visual statistical learning in infancy: evidence for a domain general learning mechanism , 2002, Cognition.

[34]  R. Aslin,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article UNSUPERVISED STATISTICAL LEARNING OF HIGHER-ORDER SPATIAL STRUCTURES FROM VISUAL SCENES , 2022 .

[35]  G. Dawson,et al.  Prototype formation in autism , 2001, Development and Psychopathology.

[36]  S. Mostofsky,et al.  Evidence for a deficit in procedural learning in children and adolescents with autism: Implications for cerebellar contribution , 2000, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[37]  E. Vogel,et al.  The visual N1 component as an index of a discrimination process. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[38]  C. Nelson,et al.  Brain activity differentiates face and object processing in 6-month-old infants. , 1999, Developmental psychology.

[39]  Elizabeth K. Johnson,et al.  Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults , 1999, Cognition.

[40]  J. Coull Neural correlates of attention and arousal: insights from electrophysiology, functional neuroimaging and psychopharmacology , 1998, Progress in Neurobiology.

[41]  C. Nelson,et al.  Recognition of the mother's face by six-month-old infants: a neurobehavioral study. , 1997, Child development.

[42]  E. Newport,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article INCIDENTAL LANGUAGE LEARNING: Ustening (and Learning) out of the Comer of Your Ear , 2022 .

[43]  R N Aslin,et al.  Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996, Science.

[44]  Shin-ichi Komatsu,et al.  Age-related and intelligence-related differences in implicit memory: effects of generation on a word-fragment completion test. , 1996, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[45]  L. Squire,et al.  Parallel brain systems for learning with and without awareness. , 1994, Learning & memory.

[46]  T W Picton,et al.  The P300 Wave of the Human Event‐Related Potential , 1992, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[47]  E Donchin,et al.  A metric for thought: a comparison of P300 latency and reaction time. , 1981, Science.

[48]  L. Squire,et al.  Preserved learning and retention of pattern-analyzing skill in amnesia: dissociation of knowing how and knowing that. , 1980, Science.

[49]  Axel Cleeremans Consciousness: the radical plasticity thesis. , 2008, Progress in brain research.

[50]  W. Alexander American psychiatric association. , 2008, P & T : a peer-reviewed journal for formulary management.

[51]  D. Tucker,et al.  Spatial sampling and filtering of EEG with spline Laplacians to estimate cortical potentials , 2005, Brain Topography.

[52]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  Intuition: a social cognitive neuroscience approach. , 2000, Psychological bulletin.

[53]  F. Conners,et al.  Implicit and explicit memory in individuals with mental retardation. , 1998, American journal of mental retardation : AJMR.

[54]  W. Perrig,et al.  IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY IN MENTALLY RETARDED, LEARNING DISABLED, AND NORMAL CHILDREN , 1995 .