Anxiety and selective attention to threat in tactical decision-making

Abstract This study investigated effects of anxiety on tactical decision-making. Trait and state anxiety were compared as predictors of bias in information search and decision-making, using a simulated search-and-rescue task. A mood induction was also used to manipulate anxiety. Results suggested that anxiety biased decision-making, but the effects were subtle, and depended on the anxiety variable involved. Participants were generally willing to accept the possibility of a low-probability large loss, but anxious mood induction reduced this bias. Trait anxiety was associated with a bias in information search, favoring sampling of information about possible losses, but only with a neutral mood induction. Anxiety may not be associated with a general, automatic bias towards threat on this task; instead, anxiety effects may vary with the affective context for information search.

[1]  D. Segal,et al.  Comprehensive handbook of psychological assessment, Vol. 2: Personality assessment. , 2004 .

[2]  Michel Hersen,et al.  Comprehensive handbook of psychological assessment , 2004 .

[3]  I. Deary,et al.  Personality Traits - 3rd edition , 2009 .

[4]  P. Hancock,et al.  Human Mental Workload , 1988 .

[5]  A. Wells,et al.  Attention and Emotion: A Clinical Perspective , 1994 .

[6]  Jan Seifert,et al.  The Relationship of Cortical Activity and Personality in a Reinforced Go-Nogo Paradigm , 2005 .

[7]  M. Eysenck,et al.  Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. , 2007, Emotion.

[8]  Eva Hudlicka Reasons for Emotions , 2007 .

[9]  Eva Hudlicka,et al.  Beyond Cognition: Modeling Emotion in Cognitive Architectures , 2004, ICCM.

[10]  Catalina J. Hooper,et al.  Personality correlates of Iowa Gambling Task performance in healthy adolescents , 2008 .

[11]  A. Tversky,et al.  Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases , 1974, Science.

[12]  J. Mayer,et al.  Mood inductions for four specific moods: A procedure employing guided imagery vignettes with music , 1995 .

[13]  Christopher K. Hsee,et al.  Risk as Feelings , 2001, Psychological bulletin.

[14]  G. Clore,et al.  Mood as Input: A Configural View of Mood Effects , 2001 .

[15]  G. Davey,et al.  Anxiety and disgust: Evidence for a unidirectional relationship , 2005 .

[16]  Y. Bar-Haim,et al.  Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. , 2007, Psychological bulletin.

[17]  David A. Lagnado,et al.  Straight Choices: The Psychology of Decision Making , 2007 .

[18]  Boris Egloff,et al.  Interactive effects of state anxiety and trait anxiety on emotional Stroop interference , 2001 .

[19]  Robin L. Nabi,et al.  Exploring the Framing Effects of Emotion , 2003, Commun. Res..

[20]  D. French,et al.  Having a Cyberball: Using a ball-throwing game as an experimental social stressor to examine the relationship between neuroticism and coping , 2009 .

[21]  P. Corr The Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory of Personality: Contents , 2008 .

[22]  Gerald Matthews,et al.  Dynamic relationships between stress states and working memory , 2010 .

[23]  A. Colley,et al.  Acquisition and performance of cognitive skills , 1989 .

[24]  W. Roth,et al.  Selective attention in anxiety: Distraction and enhancement in visual search , 2003, Depression and anxiety.

[25]  C. MacLeod,et al.  Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders. , 2005, Annual review of clinical psychology.