The control of attention and actions: Current research and future developments

In this introductory article of the special issue of Brain Research, we first present an overview of some general questions relating to cognitive control. For instance, one of the questions that remain to be answered is what control mechanisms and their neural underpinnings really 'do', beyond what is done by more basic 'computational' or data processing systems in the brain. We then briefly describe the four major issues addressed in the separate articles of this issue, namely attentional orienting, task set switching, performance and error monitoring, and response inhibition. In conclusion, we focus on some new methodological directions and illustrate how the study of cognitive control may be augmented by relatively newly emerging theoretical and empirical perspectives.

[1]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. , 2002, Psychological review.

[2]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  Intuition: a social cognitive neuroscience approach. , 2000, Psychological bulletin.

[3]  T. Goldberg,et al.  Genes and the parsing of cognitive processes , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[4]  R. Adolphs Cognitive neuroscience: Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[5]  E. Crone,et al.  Sensitivity to interference and response contingencies in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. , 2003, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[6]  A. Engel,et al.  Trial-by-Trial Coupling of Concurrent Electroencephalogram and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Identifies the Dynamics of Performance Monitoring , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[7]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.

[8]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Alterations in the BOLD fMRI signal with ageing and disease: a challenge for neuroimaging , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[9]  A. Diamond Close interrelation of motor development and cognitive development and of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. , 2000, Child development.

[10]  P. Nunez,et al.  On the Relationship of Synaptic Activity to Macroscopic Measurements: Does Co-Registration of EEG with fMRI Make Sense? , 2004, Brain Topography.

[11]  S. Blakemore,et al.  Social cognitive neuroscience: where are we heading? , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[12]  G. Glover,et al.  Reflecting upon Feelings: An fMRI Study of Neural Systems Supporting the Attribution of Emotion to Self and Other , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[13]  Falk Minow,et al.  Evaluation of neuromotor deficits in children with autism and children with a specific speech and language disorder , 2002, European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry.

[14]  Kevin A. Pelphrey,et al.  Grasping the Intentions of Others: The Perceived Intentionality of an Action Influences Activity in the Superior Temporal Sulcus during Social Perception , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[15]  Kenneth Hugdahl,et al.  Assessing the spatiotemporal evolution of neuronal activation with single-trial event-related potentials and functional MRI. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[16]  M. Sabbagh,et al.  Understanding orbitofrontal contributions to theory-of-mind reasoning: Implications for autism , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[17]  G. Mangun,et al.  Dissociating top-down attentional control from selective perception and action , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  S. Makeig,et al.  Mining event-related brain dynamics , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[19]  T. Hare,et al.  Changes in cerebral functional organization during cognitive development , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[20]  Kirsten G. Volz,et al.  A common neural system signaling the need for behavioral changes , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[21]  B. J. Casey,et al.  Imaging the developing brain: what have we learned about cognitive development? , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[22]  Matti Laine,et al.  Frontal and Temporal Dopamine Release during Working Memory and Attention Tasks in Healthy Humans: a Positron Emission Tomography Study Using the High-Affinity Dopamine D2 Receptor Ligand [11C]FLB 457 , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  U. Castiello Understanding other people's actions: intention and attention. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The Neural Basis of Economic Decision-Making in the Ultimatum Game , 2003, Science.

[25]  W. Hulstijn,et al.  Drug-induced stimulation and suppression of action monitoring in healthy volunteers , 2004, Psychopharmacology.

[26]  M. Egan,et al.  Effect of Catechol-O-Methyltransferase val158met Genotype on Attentional Control , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  The emergence of social cognitive neuroscience. , 2001, The American psychologist.

[28]  宁北芳,et al.  疟原虫var基因转换速率变化导致抗原变异[英]/Paul H, Robert P, Christodoulou Z, et al//Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A , 2005 .

[29]  M. Brass,et al.  Imitation: is cognitive neuroscience solving the correspondence problem? , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[30]  Paul D. Kieffaber,et al.  Haloperidol Impairs Learning and Error-related Negativity in Humans , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[31]  Antoni Rodríguez-Fornells,et al.  Noradrenergic Stimulation Enhances Human Action Monitoring , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  J Leon Kenemans,et al.  Spatio-temporal dynamics of top-down control: directing attention to location and/or color as revealed by ERPs and source modeling. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[33]  Y. Munakata,et al.  Developmental cognitive neuroscience: progress and potential , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[34]  N. Logothetis The neural basis of the blood-oxygen-level-dependent functional magnetic resonance imaging signal. , 2002, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[35]  T. Robbins,et al.  Chemistry of the adaptive mind , 2004, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series A: Mathematical, Physical and Engineering Sciences.

[36]  Albert Kok,et al.  Caffeine strengthens action monitoring: evidence from the error-related negativity. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[37]  E. Darcy Burgund,et al.  Comparison of functional activation foci in children and adults using a common stereotactic space , 2003, NeuroImage.

[38]  J. Schall,et al.  Performance monitoring by the supplementary eye ® eld , 2000 .

[39]  D. Yves von Cramon,et al.  Why am I unsure? Internal and external attributions of uncertainty dissociated by fMRI , 2004, NeuroImage.

[40]  M. Egan,et al.  Catechol O-methyltransferase val158-met genotype and individual variation in the brain response to amphetamine , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[41]  C. Frith,et al.  Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’ , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[42]  N. Logothetis,et al.  Neurophysiological investigation of the basis of the fMRI signal , 2001, Nature.

[43]  Thérèse J. M. Overbeek,et al.  Dissociable Components of Error Processing on the Functional Significance of the Pe Vis-à-vis the Ern/ne Performance Monitoring Processes Reflected in the Ne and Pe Review of Studies That Report Both Ne and Pe: Associations and Dissociations Pharmacological Effects , 2022 .

[44]  N. Minshew,et al.  Motor Performance and Anatomic Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) of the Basal Ganglia in Autism , 2003, Journal of child neurology.

[45]  K. Richard Ridderinkhof,et al.  Alcohol Consumption Impairs Detection of Performance Errors in Mediofrontal Cortex , 2002, Science.

[46]  N. Logothetis The Underpinnings of the BOLD Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Signal , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[47]  A. Abu-Akel A neurobiological mapping of theory of mind , 2003, Brain Research Reviews.