Development of the Hippocampal Cognitive Map in Preweanling Rats

The Space in Your Head Space, and events associated with places and spaces, are represented in the brain by a circuitry made of place cells, head direction cells, grid cells, and border cells. These cell types form a collective dynamic representation of our position as we move through the environment. How this representation is formed has remained a mystery. Is it acquired, or are we born with the ability to represent external space (see the Perspective by Palmer and Lynch)? Langston et al. (p. 1576) and Wills et al. (p. 1573) investigated the early development of spatial activity in the hippocampal formation and the entorhinal cortex of rat pups when they first began to explore their environment. Rudiments of place cells, head direction cells, and grid cells already existed when the pups made their first movements out of the nest. A neural representation of external space at this early time points to strong innate components for perception of space. These findings provide experimental support for Kant's 200-year-old concept of space as an a priori faculty of the mind. Some aspects of spatial cognition develop very early and may be determined innately. Orienting in large-scale space depends on the interaction of environmental experience and preconfigured, possibly innate, constructs. Place, head-direction, and grid cells in the hippocampal formation provide allocentric representations of space. Here we show how these cognitive representations emerge and develop as rat pups first begin to explore their environment. Directional, locational, and rhythmic organization of firing are present during initial exploration, including adultlike directional firing. The stability and precision of place cell firing continue to develop throughout juvenility. Stable grid cell firing appears later but matures rapidly to adult levels. Our results demonstrate the presence of three neuronal representations of space before extensive experience and show how they develop with age.

[1]  J. O'Keefe,et al.  The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. , 1971, Brain research.

[2]  B. H. Bland,et al.  Developmental aspects of hippocampal electrical activity and motor behavior in the rat , 1979, Experimental Neurology.

[3]  R. Passingham The hippocampus as a cognitive map J. O'Keefe & L. Nadel, Oxford University Press, Oxford (1978). 570 pp., £25.00 , 1979, Neuroscience.

[4]  F. Schenk Development of place navigation in rats from weaning to puberty. , 1985, Behavioral and neural biology.

[5]  K. Cheng A purely geometric module in the rat's spatial representation , 1986, Cognition.

[6]  J. W. Rudy,et al.  Ontogeny of spatial navigation behaviors in the rat: dissociation of "proximal"- and "distal"-cue-based behaviors. , 1987, Behavioral neuroscience.

[7]  R U Muller,et al.  Head-direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats. I. Description and quantitative analysis , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[8]  AC Tose Cell , 1993, Cell.

[9]  J. O’Keefe,et al.  Phase relationship between hippocampal place units and the EEG theta rhythm , 1993, Hippocampus.

[10]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  A geometric process for spatial reorientation in young children , 1994, Nature.

[11]  C. Gerrish,et al.  Environmental temperature modulates onset of independent feeding: warmer is sooner. , 1996, Developmental psychobiology.

[12]  D. Wilkin,et al.  Neuron , 2001, Brain Research.

[13]  Thomas J. Wills,et al.  Long-term plasticity in hippocampal place-cell representation of environmental geometry , 2002, Nature.

[14]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Development of spatial firing in the hippocampus of young rats , 2002, Hippocampus.

[15]  J. Gaztelu,et al.  Changes in hippocampal cell discharge patterns and theta rhythm spectral properties as a function of walking velocity in the guinea pig , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  T. Hafting,et al.  Microstructure of a spatial map in the entorhinal cortex , 2005, Nature.

[17]  Bruce L. McNaughton,et al.  Path integration and the neural basis of the 'cognitive map' , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[18]  J. O’Keefe,et al.  An oscillatory interference model of grid cell firing , 2007, Hippocampus.

[19]  K. Jeffery,et al.  Experience-dependent rescaling of entorhinal grids , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[20]  Lisa M. Giocomo,et al.  Temporal Frequency of Subthreshold Oscillations Scales with Entorhinal Grid Cell Field Spacing , 2007, Science.

[21]  J. Knierim,et al.  Influence of boundary removal on the spatial representations of the medial entorhinal cortex , 2008, Hippocampus.

[22]  M. Moser,et al.  Representation of Geometric Borders in the Entorhinal Cortex , 2008, Science.

[23]  T. Hafting,et al.  Hippocampus-independent phase precession in entorhinal grid cells , 2008, Nature.

[24]  M. Moser,et al.  Impaired Spatial Representation in CA1 after Lesion of Direct Input from Entorhinal Cortex , 2008, Neuron.

[25]  Michael N. Economo,et al.  Development of theta rhythmicity in entorhinal stellate cells of the juvenile rat. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  E. Save,et al.  Unstable CA1 place cell representation in rats with entorhinal cortex lesions , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.

[27]  J. O’Keefe,et al.  Grid cells and theta as oscillatory interference: Electrophysiological data from freely moving rats , 2008, Hippocampus.

[28]  J. O’Keefe,et al.  Boundary Vector Cells in the Subiculum of the Hippocampal Formation , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  D. Tank,et al.  Intracellular dynamics of hippocampal place cells during virtual navigation , 2009, Nature.

[30]  R. Rosenfeld Nature , 2009, Otolaryngology--head and neck surgery : official journal of American Academy of Otolaryngology-Head and Neck Surgery.

[31]  Edvard I Moser,et al.  Development of the Spatial Representation System in the Rat , 2010, Science.