Source Confusion as an Explanation of Cultivation: A Test of the Mechanisms Underlying Confusion of Fiction with Reality on Television

Cultivation studies have found evidence that heavy television viewers adopt a world view congruent with how the world is portrayed in fictional television programs. An explanation is that viewers may remember fictional TV stories as realistic stories or news (fiction-to-news confusion). Until now, fiction-to-news confusion was found only if at least a week evolved between watching TV and asking viewers what was remembered. The present study conducted with a purposive sample of students and employees of a college in The Netherlands (N = 96; M age = 28.6 yr., SD= 10.9) indicates that fiction-to-news confusions can also occur almost immediately after watching. In addition, whereas earlier research suggests that fiction-to-news confusions are associated with heavy viewing, i.e., more confusion when more hours per day are spent on TV viewing in leisure time, and faulty memory, the present study more specifically suggests that participants make many fiction-to-news confusions when they are exposed to relatively many fictional TV fragments that contain threatening, violent events.

[1]  Robert V. Bellamy,et al.  Television and the Remote Control: Grazing on a Vast Wasteland , 1996 .

[2]  Cynthia A. Hoffner,et al.  Enjoyment of Mediated Fright and Violence: A Meta-Analysis , 2005 .

[3]  A G Greenwald,et al.  In search of reliable persuasion effects: III. The sleeper effect is dead. Long live the sleeper effect. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[4]  L. Gross,et al.  Living with television: the violence profile. , 1976, The Journal of communication.

[5]  Erica Weintraub Austin,et al.  Gender as a Variable in Interpretation of Alcohol-Related Messages , 2002, Commun. Res..

[6]  Daniel Kahneman,et al.  Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability , 1973 .

[7]  Yuki Fujioka,et al.  Emotional TV Viewing and Minority Audience , 2005, Commun. Res..

[8]  R. Greene A common basis for recency effects in immediate and delayed recall. , 1986 .

[9]  Marilyn Jackson-Beeck,et al.  TV Violence Profile No. 8: The Highlights , 1977 .

[10]  C. Hoffner Children's wishful identification and parasocial interaction with favorite television characters , 1996 .

[11]  John E. Newhagen,et al.  The Evening's Bad News: Effects of Compelling Negative Television News Images on Memory , 1992 .

[12]  Pamela J. Shoemaker,et al.  Hardwired for News: Using Biological and Cultural Evolution to Explain the Surveillance Function , 1996 .

[13]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Mental contamination and mental correction: unwanted influences on judgments and evaluations. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.

[14]  Zheng Wang,et al.  Wait! Don't Turn That Dial! More Excitement to Come! The Effects of Story Length and Production Pacing in Local Television News on Channel Changing Behavior and Information Processing in a Free Choice Environment , 2005 .

[15]  Jonathan Cohen Defining Identification: A Theoretical Look at the Identification of Audiences With Media Characters , 2001 .

[16]  Michael Morgan,et al.  Two Decades of Cultivation Research: An Appraisal and Meta-Analysis , 1997 .

[17]  L. J. Shrum,et al.  Processes and Effects in the Construction of Social Reality , 1993 .

[18]  Alan G. Sawyer,et al.  The sleeper effect in persuasion: a meta-analytic review. , 2004, Psychological bulletin.

[19]  Michael A. Shapiro,et al.  Memory and Decision Processes in the Construction of Social Reality , 1991 .

[20]  E. Austin,et al.  Effects of interpretations of televised alcohol portrayals on children's alcohol beliefs , 1994 .

[21]  Marie-Louise Mares The Role of Source Confusions in Television's Cultivation of Social Reality Judgments , 1996 .