Contributions of cat posterior parietal cortex to visuospatial discrimination

The purpose of the present study was to examine the contributions made by cat posterior parietal cortex to the analyses of the relative position of objects in visual space. Two cats were trained on a landmark task in which they learned to report the position of a landmark object relative to a right or left food-reward chamber. Subsequently, three pairs of cooling loops were implanted bilaterally in contact with visuoparietal cortices forming the crown of the middle suprasylvian gyrus (MSg; architectonic area 7) and the banks of the posterior-middle suprasylvian sulcus (pMS sulcal cortex) and in contact with the ventral-posterior suprasylvian (vPS) region of visuotemporal cortex. Bilateral deactivation of pMS sulcal cortex resulted in a profound impairment for all six tested positions of the landmark, yet bilateral deactivation of neither area 7 nor vPS cortex yielded any deficits. In control tasks (visual orienting and object discrimination), there was no evidence for any degree of attentional blindness or impairment of form discrimination during bilateral deactivation of pMS cortex. Therefore, we conclude that bilateral cooling of pMS cortex, but neither area 7 nor vPS cortex, induces a specific deficit in spatial localization as examined with the landmark task. These observations have significant bearing on our understanding of visuospatial processing in cat, monkey, and human cortices.

[1]  Stephen G. Lomber,et al.  The cryoloop: an adaptable reversible cooling deactivation method for behavioral or electrophysiological assessment of neural function , 1999, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[2]  Bertram R Payne,et al.  A method to assess the functional impact of cerebral connections on target populations of neurons , 1999, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[3]  Stephen G Lomber,et al.  The advantages and limitations of permanent or reversible deactivation techniques in the assessment of neural function , 1999, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[4]  R. Nudo,et al.  Dissociation of visual and auditory pattern discrimination functions within the cat's temporal cortex. , 1998, Behavioral neuroscience.

[5]  S G Lomber,et al.  Removal of two halves restores the whole: Reversal of visual hemineglect during bilateral cortical or collicular inactivation in the cat , 1996, Visual Neuroscience.

[6]  B. Payne,et al.  Age dependent modification of cytochrome oxidase activity in the cat dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus following removal of primary visual cortex , 1996, Visual Neuroscience.

[7]  S G Lomber,et al.  Perceptual and cognitive visual functions of parietal and temporal cortices in the cat. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[8]  S G Lomber,et al.  Learning and recall of form discriminations during reversible cooling deactivation of ventral-posterior suprasylvian cortex in the cat. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[9]  S G Lomber,et al.  Reversible visual hemineglect. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[10]  R. Adams,et al.  Stroke in Children and Young Adults , 1995 .

[11]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  ‘What’ and ‘where’ in the human brain , 1994, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[12]  K. Heilman,et al.  Posterior neocortical systems subserving awareness and neglect. Neglect associated with superior temporal sulcus but not area 7 lesions. , 1994, Archives of neurology.

[13]  W. Merigan,et al.  Motion perception following lesions of the superior temporal sulcus in the monkey. , 1994, Cerebral cortex.

[14]  S G Lomber,et al.  Reversible inactivation of visual processing operations in middle suprasylvian cortex of the behaving cat. , 1994, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[15]  K. Krüger,et al.  Lesion of the suprasylvian cortex impairs depth perception of cats. , 1993, Neuroreport.

[16]  N Mai,et al.  Disturbance of movement vision after bilateral posterior brain damage. Further evidence and follow up observations. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[17]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Recovery of function after lesions in the superior temporal sulcus in the monkey. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Dissociation of object and spatial visual processing pathways in human extrastriate cortex. , 1991, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[19]  G. Ettlinger “Object Vision” and “Spatial Vision”: The Neuropsychological Evidence for the Distinction , 1990, Cortex.

[20]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Deficits in speed discrimination following lesions of the lateral suprasylvian cortex in the cat , 1989, Visual Neuroscience.

[21]  W. Newsome,et al.  A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT) , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[22]  C. R. Olson,et al.  Cortical and subcortical afferent connections of a posterior division of feline area 7 (Area 7p) , 1987, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[23]  David Gaffan,et al.  Visual Identification following Inferotemporal Ablation in the Monkey , 1986, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[24]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Contribution of striate inputs to the visuospatial functions of parieto-preoccipital cortex in monkeys , 1982, Behavioural Brain Research.

[25]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Equivalence of parieto-preoccipital subareas for visuospatial ability in monkeys , 1982, Behavioural Brain Research.

[26]  L. Palmer,et al.  Retinotopic organization of areas 20 and 21 in the cat , 1980, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[27]  Michael Petrides,et al.  Restricted posterior parietal lesions in the rhesus monkey and performance on visuospatial tasks , 1979, Brain Research.

[28]  L. Palmer,et al.  The retinotopic organization of lateral suprasylvian visual areas in the cat , 1978, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[29]  R. K. Thomas,et al.  Effects of posterior parietal and frontal neocortical lesions in the squirrel monkey. , 1975, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[30]  J. Simpson CENTRAL PROCESSING OF VISUAL INFORMATION PART B: VISUAL CENTRES IN THE BRAIN , 1974 .

[31]  W. Pohl,et al.  Dissociation of spatial discrimination deficits following frontal and parietal lesions in monkeys. , 1973, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[32]  C. A. Marsan Topographischer Hirnatlas der Katze für experimental-physiologische Untersuchungen , 1963 .

[33]  G. Ettlinger,et al.  Posterior biparietal ablations in the monkey. Changes to neurological and behavioral testing. , 1960, Archives of neurology.

[34]  E. De Renzi Disorders of Visual Recognition , 2000, Seminars in neurology.

[35]  M. Goodale,et al.  The visual brain in action , 1995 .

[36]  B R Payne,et al.  Evidence for visual cortical area homologs in cat and macaque monkey. , 1993, Cerebral cortex.

[37]  J. Lynch,et al.  Deficits of visual attention and saccadic eye movements after lesions of parietooccipital cortex in monkeys. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  Leslie G. Ungerleider Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .

[39]  C. Gross Visual Functions of Inferotemporal Cortex , 1973 .