What's in a Day? A Guide to Decomposing the Variance in Intensive Longitudinal Data

In recent years there has been a growing interest in the use of intensive longitudinal research designs to study within-person processes. Examples are studies that use experience sampling data and autoregressive modeling to investigate emotion dynamics and between-person differences therein. Such designs often involve multiple measurements per day and multiple days per person, and it is not clear how this nesting of the data should be accounted for: That is, should such data be considered as two-level data (which is common practice at this point), with occasions nested in persons, or as three-level data with beeps nested in days which are nested in persons. We show that a significance test of the day-level variance in an empty three-level model is not reliable when there is autocorrelation. Furthermore, we show that misspecifying the number of levels can lead to spurious or misleading findings, such as inflated variance or autoregression estimates. Throughout the paper we present instructions and R code for the implementation of the proposed models, which includes a novel three-level AR(1) model that estimates moment-to-moment inertia and day-to-day inertia. Based on our simulations we recommend model selection using autoregressive multilevel models in combination with the AIC. We illustrate this method using empirical emotion data from two independent samples, and discuss the implications and the relevance of the existence of a day level for the field.

[1]  Ellen L. Hamaker,et al.  UvA-DARE ( Digital Academic Repository ) To center or not to center ? Investigating inertia with a multilevel autoregressive model , 2014 .

[2]  J. Berkhof,et al.  Effects of stressful daily events on mood states: relationship to global perceived stress. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[3]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[4]  S. Mineka,et al.  Within-person variations in self-focused attention and negative affect in depression and anxiety: A diary study , 2010 .

[5]  E. Hamaker,et al.  Investigating inter-individual differences in short-term intra-individual variability. , 2012, Psychological methods.

[6]  T. Trull,et al.  Using experience sampling methods/ecological momentary assessment (ESM/EMA) in clinical assessment and clinical research: introduction to the special section. , 2009, Psychological assessment.

[7]  Lijuan Peggy Wang,et al.  On disaggregating between-person and within-person effects with longitudinal data using multilevel models. , 2015, Psychological methods.

[8]  Sy-Miin Chow,et al.  Examining Interindividual Differences in Cyclicity of Pleasant and Unpleasant Affects Using Spectral Analysis and Item Response Modeling , 2005 .

[9]  H. Akaike A new look at the statistical model identification , 1974 .

[10]  P. Kuppens,et al.  Emotional Inertia and Psychological Maladjustment , 2010, Psychological science.

[11]  Ulrich Ebner-Priemer,et al.  Ambulatory assessment. , 2013, Annual review of clinical psychology.

[12]  E. Adam,et al.  Loneliness and cortisol: Momentary, day-to-day, and trait associations , 2010, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[13]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Duration neglect in retrospective evaluations of affective episodes. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[14]  P. Brockhoff,et al.  lmerTest: Tests for random and fixed effects for linear mixed effect models (lmer objects of lme4 package) , 2014 .

[15]  Ellen L. Hamaker,et al.  Regime Switching State-Space Models Applied to Psychological Processes: Handling Missing Data and Making Inferences , 2012 .

[16]  D. Hall Models for Intensive Longitudinal Data , 2007 .

[17]  G. Schwarz Estimating the Dimension of a Model , 1978 .

[18]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Estimation and interpretation of 1/fα noise in human cognition , 2004 .

[19]  Eva Ceulemans,et al.  Modeling Affect Dynamics: State of the Art and Future Challenges , 2015 .

[20]  P. Kuppens,et al.  Emotional inertia prospectively predicts the onset of depressive disorder in adolescence. , 2012, Emotion.

[21]  Peter Kuppens,et al.  The dynamic interplay between emotions in daily life: augmentation, blunting, and the role of appraisal overlap. , 2012, Emotion.

[22]  Per B. Brockhoff,et al.  Automated mixed ANOVA modeling of sensory and consumer data , 2015 .

[23]  Peter Kuppens,et al.  Correction: A Network Approach to Psychopathology: New Insights into Clinical Longitudinal Data , 2014, PLoS ONE.

[24]  M. Houben,et al.  The relation between short-term emotion dynamics and psychological well-being: A meta-analysis. , 2015, Psychological bulletin.

[25]  Edward R. Watkins,et al.  Ruminative Self-Focus and Negative Affect , 2008, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[26]  P. Kuppens,et al.  Changing emotion dynamics: individual differences in the effect of anticipatory social stress on emotional inertia. , 2012, Emotion.

[27]  P. Links,et al.  Variability and predictors of negative mood intensity in patients with borderline personality disorder and recurrent suicidal behavior: multilevel analyses applied to experience sampling methodology. , 2010, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[28]  Daniel D. Blaine,et al.  Modeling unipolar depression as a chaotic process. , 2003, Psychological assessment.

[29]  P. Kuppens,et al.  Emotional inertia contributes to depressive symptoms beyond perseverative thinking , 2015, Cognition & emotion.

[30]  P. Kuppens,et al.  Affect dynamics in relation to depressive symptoms: variable, unstable or inert? , 2013, Emotion.

[31]  J. Suls,et al.  Emotional Reactivity to Everyday Problems, Affective Inertia, and Neuroticism , 1998 .

[32]  Philippe Delespaul,et al.  Diurnal mood variation in major depressive disorder. , 2006, Emotion.

[33]  D. Borsboom,et al.  Network analysis: an integrative approach to the structure of psychopathology. , 2013, Annual review of clinical psychology.

[34]  D. Borsboom,et al.  A Network Approach to Psychopathology: New Insights into Clinical Longitudinal Data , 2013, PloS one.