Color category influences heterogeneous visual search for color.

Many previous studies have revealed chromatic characteristics of visual search using relatively simple stimuli. They suggested that color difference between a target and distractors was a crucial factor. However, it may not be applicable to natural environments that contain numerous colors. This study demonstrates the existence of a color-category effect on heterochromatic visual search. Color differences between a target and distractors were constant in the OSA uniform color scales; however, the search times varied widely. This suggests that color differences alone do not explain search performance. To clarify the mediation of a higher-order categorical color process, search times were analyzed using 11 basic colors. When the color category of a target was shared by a larger number of distractors, the search performance declined. However, when the color category of a target was not shared with distractors, the target was easily detected. The results suggest that heterochromatic stimuli could be segregated by categorical color perception.

[1]  T. Indow A test of uniformities in the OSA‐UCS and the NCS , 2003 .

[2]  Harvey S. Smallman,et al.  Category effects in color memory , 1989 .

[3]  香川高弘 Locating Basic Colors in the OSA Space , 1987 .

[4]  P. Lennie,et al.  Color search and visual field segregation , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[5]  Robert M. Boynton,et al.  Salience of chromatic basic color terms confirmed by three measures , 1990, Vision Research.

[6]  H. Smallman,et al.  Segregation of basic colors in an information display. , 1990, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[7]  A Treisman,et al.  Feature analysis in early vision: evidence from search asymmetries. , 1988, Psychological review.

[8]  Preeti Verghese,et al.  The psychophysics of visual search , 2000, Vision Research.

[9]  Robert L. Goldstone Influences of categorization on perceptual discrimination. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[10]  H Pashler,et al.  Cross-dimensional interaction and texture segregation , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.

[11]  Michael D'Zmura,et al.  Color in visual search , 1991, Vision Research.

[12]  A L Nagy,et al.  The achromatic mechanism and mechanisms tuned to chromaticity and luminance in visual search. , 2000, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[13]  S Yamane,et al.  Color selectivity of neurons in the inferior temporal cortex of the awake macaque monkey , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[14]  J. Wolfe,et al.  Guided Search 2.0 A revised model of visual search , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[15]  A L Nagy,et al.  Interactions between achromatic and chromatic mechanisms in visual search , 1999, Vision Research.

[16]  W. Cowan,et al.  Distractor Heterogeneity versus Linear Separability in Colour Visual Search , 1996 .

[17]  A. Treisman,et al.  A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[18]  Allen L. Nagy,et al.  Distractor heterogeneity, attention, and color in visual search , 2003, Vision Research.

[19]  R. E. Christ Review and Analysis of Color Coding Research for Visual Displays , 1975 .

[20]  P. Kay,et al.  Basic Color Terms: Their Universality and Evolution , 1973 .

[21]  Jack L. Vevea,et al.  Why do categories affect stimulus judgment? , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[22]  D. J. Felleman,et al.  A spatially organized representation of colour in macaque cortical area V2 , 2003, Nature.

[23]  R. Vautin,et al.  Neuronal mechanisms of color categorization in areas V1, V2 and V4 of macaque monkey visual cortex , 1996, Behavioural Brain Research.

[24]  J. Davidoff,et al.  The categorical perception of colors and facial expressions: The effect of verbal interference , 2000, Memory & cognition.

[25]  R. M. Boynton,et al.  Categorical color perception of Japanese observers: Comparison with that of Americans , 1987, Vision Research.

[26]  W. Cowan,et al.  Visual search for colour targets that are or are not linearly separable from distractors , 1996, Vision Research.

[27]  A. Nagy,et al.  Chromaticity and Luminance as Coding Dimensions in Visual Search , 1992, Human factors.

[28]  D. L. Macadam Uniform color scales. , 1974, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[29]  J. Kulikowski,et al.  Wavelength discrimination at detection threshold. , 1990, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[30]  Martin Arguin,et al.  Conjunction and linear non-separability effects in visual shape encoding , 2000, Vision Research.

[31]  H. Komatsu Mechanisms of central color vision , 1998, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[32]  Darren Van Laar,et al.  The structure of colour naming space , 2000, Vision Research.

[33]  William B. Cowan,et al.  Convex hull test of the linear separability hypothesis in visual search , 1999, Vision Research.

[34]  H. Egeth,et al.  Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[35]  Harvey S. Smallman,et al.  On the usefulness of basic colour coding in an information display , 1993 .

[36]  R C Carter,et al.  Color and conspicuousness. , 1981, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[37]  Ellen C. Carter,et al.  Color coding for rapid location of small symbols , 1988 .

[38]  John R. Anderson,et al.  The Adaptive Nature of Human Categorization. , 1991 .

[39]  A. Nagy,et al.  Critical color differences determined with a visual search task. , 1990, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[40]  J. Bruner On perceptual readiness. , 1957, Psychological review.

[41]  G W Humphreys,et al.  Driving attention with the top down: The relative contribution of target templates to the linear separability effect in the size dimension , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.