Predictive models avoid excessive reductionism in cognitive neuroimaging

Understanding the organization of complex behavior as it relates to the brain requires modeling the behavior, the relevant mental processes, and the corresponding neural activity. Experiments in cognitive neuroscience typically study a psychological process via controlled manipulations, reducing behavior to one of its components. Such reductionism can easily lead to paradigm-bound theories. Predictive models can generalize brain-mind associations to arbitrary new tasks and stimuli. We argue that they are needed to broaden theories beyond specific paradigms. Predicting behavior from neural activity can support robust reverse inference, isolating brain structures that support particular mental processes. The converse prediction enables modeling brain responses as a function of a complete description of the task, rather than building on oppositions.

[1]  Yoshua Bengio,et al.  Generative Adversarial Nets , 2014, NIPS.

[2]  J. Gallant,et al.  Complete functional characterization of sensory neurons by system identification. , 2006, Annual review of neuroscience.

[3]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Fusiform Face Area: A Module in Human Extrastriate Cortex Specialized for Face Perception , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  Russell A. Poldrack,et al.  Discovering Relations Between Mind, Brain, and Mental Disorders Using Topic Mapping , 2012, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[5]  M. A. MacIver,et al.  Neuroscience Needs Behavior: Correcting a Reductionist Bias , 2017, Neuron.

[6]  Michael Eickenberg,et al.  Seeing it all: Convolutional network layers map the function of the human visual system , 2017, NeuroImage.

[7]  Jack L. Gallant,et al.  Encoding and decoding in fMRI , 2011, NeuroImage.

[8]  R. Poldrack,et al.  From Brain Maps to Cognitive Ontologies: Informatics and the Search for Mental Structure. , 2016, Annual review of psychology.

[9]  Jonathan W. Pillow,et al.  Discovering Event Structure in Continuous Narrative Perception and Memory , 2016, Neuron.

[10]  Jean-Baptiste Poline,et al.  Atlases of cognition with large-scale human brain mapping , 2018, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[11]  J. DiCarlo,et al.  Using goal-driven deep learning models to understand sensory cortex , 2016, Nature Neuroscience.

[12]  Miguel A. L. Nicolelis,et al.  Brain–machine interfaces: past, present and future , 2006, Trends in Neurosciences.

[13]  Masa-aki Sato,et al.  Visual Image Reconstruction from Human Brain Activity using a Combination of Multiscale Local Image Decoders , 2008, Neuron.

[14]  Russell A. Poldrack,et al.  Text to brain: predicting the spatial distribution of neuroimaging observations from text reports , 2018, MICCAI.

[15]  Christopher L. Asplund,et al.  Functional Specialization and Flexibility in Human Association Cortex. , 2016, Cerebral cortex.

[16]  Tom M. Mitchell,et al.  Machine learning classifiers and fMRI: A tutorial overview , 2009, NeuroImage.

[17]  Russell A. Poldrack,et al.  Handbook of Functional MRI Data Analysis: Visualizing, localizing, and reporting fMRI data , 2011 .

[18]  Jessica A. Turner,et al.  The Cognitive Paradigm Ontology: Design and Application , 2011, Neuroinformatics.

[19]  Antal van den Bosch,et al.  Prediction During Natural Language Comprehension. , 2016, Cerebral cortex.

[20]  Bertrand Thirion,et al.  How machine learning is shaping cognitive neuroimaging , 2014, GigaScience.

[21]  Tonio Ball,et al.  Causal interpretation rules for encoding and decoding models in neuroimaging , 2015, NeuroImage.

[22]  T. Yarkoni,et al.  Choosing Prediction Over Explanation in Psychology: Lessons From Machine Learning , 2017, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[23]  A. Newell You can't play 20 questions with nature and win : projective comments on the papers of this symposium , 1973 .

[24]  B T Thomas Yeo,et al.  Data-Driven Extraction of a Nested Model of Human Brain Function , 2017, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  R. Poldrack Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data? , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[26]  Timothy N. Rubin,et al.  Decoding brain activity using a large-scale probabilistic functional-anatomical atlas of human cognition , 2016 .

[27]  Brian Murphy,et al.  Simultaneously Uncovering the Patterns of Brain Regions Involved in Different Story Reading Subprocesses , 2014, PloS one.

[28]  Daniela M. Witten,et al.  An Introduction to Statistical Learning: with Applications in R , 2013 .

[29]  R. Poldrack Inferring Mental States from Neuroimaging Data: From Reverse Inference to Large-Scale Decoding , 2011, Neuron.

[30]  Marcel A. J. van Gerven,et al.  Deep Neural Networks Reveal a Gradient in the Complexity of Neural Representations across the Ventral Stream , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  J. Gallant,et al.  Identifying natural images from human brain activity , 2008, Nature.

[32]  Vaidehi S. Natu,et al.  Category-Specific Cortical Activity Precedes Retrieval During Memory Search , 2005, Science.

[33]  Lars Hausfeld,et al.  Acoustic and higher-level representations of naturalistic auditory scenes in human auditory and frontal cortex , 2018, NeuroImage.

[34]  Laura Gwilliams,et al.  Performance-optimized hierarchical models only partially predict neural responses during perceptual decision making , 2017, bioRxiv.

[35]  Ha Hong,et al.  Performance-optimized hierarchical models predict neural responses in higher visual cortex , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[36]  Jeffrey Dean,et al.  Distributed Representations of Words and Phrases and their Compositionality , 2013, NIPS.

[37]  N. Logothetis,et al.  Shape representation in the inferior temporal cortex of monkeys , 1995, Current Biology.

[38]  Tom Michael Mitchell,et al.  Predicting Human Brain Activity Associated with the Meanings of Nouns , 2008, Science.

[39]  Trevor Hastie,et al.  An Introduction to Statistical Learning , 2013, Springer Texts in Statistics.

[40]  N. Lazar,et al.  The ASA Statement on p-Values: Context, Process, and Purpose , 2016 .

[41]  Essa Yacoub,et al.  Reconstructing the spectrotemporal modulations of real-life sounds from fMRI response patterns , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[42]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields of single neurones in the cat's striate cortex , 1959, The Journal of physiology.

[43]  Alexander G. Huth,et al.  Attention During Natural Vision Warps Semantic Representation Across the Human Brain , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.

[44]  Aniket Kittur,et al.  The Cognitive Atlas: Toward a Knowledge Foundation for Cognitive Neuroscience , 2011, Front. Neuroinform..

[45]  K. Grill-Spector,et al.  The human visual cortex. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[46]  Thomas L. Griffiths,et al.  Supplementary Information for Natural Speech Reveals the Semantic Maps That Tile Human Cerebral Cortex , 2022 .

[47]  Russell A. Poldrack,et al.  Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data , 2011, Nature Methods.

[48]  V. Michel,et al.  Recruitment of an Area Involved in Eye Movements During Mental Arithmetic , 2009, Science.

[49]  Daniel L. K. Yamins,et al.  A Task-Optimized Neural Network Replicates Human Auditory Behavior, Predicts Brain Responses, and Reveals a Cortical Processing Hierarchy , 2018, Neuron.

[50]  Nikolaus Kriegeskorte,et al.  Deep Supervised, but Not Unsupervised, Models May Explain IT Cortical Representation , 2014, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[51]  David J. Field,et al.  Emergence of simple-cell receptive field properties by learning a sparse code for natural images , 1996, Nature.

[52]  M. Lindquist,et al.  An fMRI-based neurologic signature of physical pain. , 2013, The New England journal of medicine.

[53]  A. Ishai,et al.  Distributed and Overlapping Representations of Faces and Objects in Ventral Temporal Cortex , 2001, Science.