Overcoming the planning fallacy through willpower : effects of implementation intentions on actual and predicted task-completion times

Previous work has shown that people often underestimate their task-completion times (Buehler, Griffin, & Ross, 1994). The present research examined whether this optimistic bias may be reduced through the formation of implementation intentions. In an experimental study, participants were requested to complete an assignment within a specified time period. Half of these participants made implementation intentions about where and when they would complete the assignment. The remaining participants were simply given the goal of completing the assignment. The results showed that furnishing participants' goals with implementation intentions led to (a) more optimistic completion predictions, (b) an even greater increase in actual rates of goal completion, and, consequently, (c) a significant reduction in optimistic bias in completion predictions. Furthermore, the reduction in optimistic bias among implementation–intention participants was found to be mediated by a smaller number of interruptions while working on the assignment. Together, these findings attest to the importance of implementation planning in overcoming unrealistic optimism in task-completion predictions. Copyright © 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

[1]  D. Wegner,et al.  Apparent mental causation. Sources of the experience of will. , 1999, The American psychologist.

[2]  P. Gollwitzer Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. , 1999 .

[3]  P. Gollwitzer Action phases and mind-sets , 1990 .

[4]  R. Buehler,et al.  Exploring the "planning fallacy": Why people underestimate their task completion times. , 1994 .

[5]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Situated Optimism: Specific Outcome Expectancies and Self-Regulation , 1998 .

[6]  D. A. Kenny,et al.  The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[7]  P. Gollwitzer,et al.  Effects of failure on subsequent performance: the importance of self-defining goals. , 1996, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[8]  P. Sheeran,et al.  Implementation Intentions and the Theory of Planned Behavior , 1997, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[9]  N. Ach,et al.  Über den Willensakt und das Temperament : eine experimentelle Untersuchung , 1910 .

[10]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Effects of mindset on positive illusions. , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[11]  Jürgen Beckmann,et al.  Deliberative versus implemental states of mind: The issue of impartiality in predecisional and postdecisional information processing , 1987 .

[12]  R. Baumeister,et al.  Self-control as limited resource: regulatory depletion patterns. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[13]  P. Gollwitzer,et al.  The Willful Pursuit of Identity , 1998 .

[14]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  Motivated closing of the mind: "seizing" and "freezing". , 1996, Psychological review.

[15]  D. Funder,et al.  Congruence of others' and self-judgments of personality. , 1997 .

[16]  N. Ach,et al.  Uber den Willensakt und das Temperament. , 1910 .

[17]  H. Heckhausen,et al.  Deliberative and implemental mind-sets: Cognitive tuning toward congruous thoughts and information , 1990 .

[18]  P. Gollwitzer The Volitional Benefits of Planning , 1996 .

[19]  J. Carroll The Effect of Imagining an Event on Expectations for the Event: An Interpretation in Terms of the Availability Heuristic. , 1978 .

[20]  Sydney Gregory,et al.  Great planning disasters , 1982 .

[21]  Jutta Heckhausen,et al.  Motivation and action , 1991 .

[22]  R. Baumeister,et al.  Ego depletion: is the active self a limited resource? , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[23]  Peter M. Gollwitzer,et al.  Effects of deliberative and implemental mind-sets on illusion of control. , 1989 .

[24]  B. Verplanken,et al.  Good intentions, bad habits, and effects of forming implementation intentions on healthy eating , 1999 .

[25]  R. Buehler,et al.  It's About Time: Optimistic Predictions in Work and Love , 1995 .

[26]  S. Sherman On the self-erasing nature of errors of prediction. , 1980 .

[27]  L. Ross,et al.  The role of construal processes in overconfident predictions about the self and others. , 1990, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[28]  R. Baumeister,et al.  When ego threats lead to self-regulation failure: negative consequences of high self-esteem. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[29]  T. Goschke,et al.  Representation of intentions: Persisting activation in memory. , 1993 .

[30]  P. Gollwitzer,et al.  Implementation intentions and effective goal pursuit , 1997 .

[31]  J. Metcalfe,et al.  A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: dynamics of willpower. , 1999, Psychological review.

[32]  C. Midden,et al.  To plan or not to plan? Goal achievement or interrupting the performance of mundane behaviors , 1999 .

[33]  P. Gollwitzer Goal Achievement: The Role of Intentions , 1993 .

[34]  W. Mischel,et al.  Effects of temptation-inhibiting and task-facilitating plans on self-control. , 1976 .

[35]  J. Kruger,et al.  Unskilled and unaware of it: how difficulties in recognizing one's own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[36]  Steven J. Sherman,et al.  The effects of explaining hypothetical future events: From possibility to probability to actuality and beyond , 1981 .

[37]  P. Sheeran,et al.  Implementation intentions and repeated behaviour: augmenting the predictive validity of the theory of planned behaviour , 1999 .

[38]  R. Buehler,et al.  The Role of Motivated Reasoning in Optimistic Time Predictions , 1997 .

[39]  H. Aarts,et al.  Habits as knowledge structures: automaticity in goal-directed behavior. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[40]  H. Heckhausen,et al.  Thought contents and cognitive functioning in motivational versus volitional states of mind , 1987 .

[41]  H. Aarts,et al.  Habits as knowledge structures: Automaticity in goal-directed behavior , 2000 .

[42]  Z. Kunda,et al.  The case for motivated reasoning. , 1990, Psychological bulletin.

[43]  P. Gollwitzer,et al.  Metacognition in Action: The Importance of Implementation Intentions , 1998, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[44]  Derek J. Koehler,et al.  Explanation, imagination, and confidence in judgment. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[45]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Mental simulation, motivation, and action. , 1996 .

[46]  J. Kemuma The past and the future in the present , 2000 .

[47]  Sander L. Koole,et al.  On the Seizing and Freezing of Negotiator Inferences: Need for Cognitive Closure Moderates the Use of Heuristics in Negotiation , 1999 .

[48]  A. Tversky,et al.  Intuitive Prediction: Biases and Corrective Procedures , 1982 .

[49]  V. Murgraff,et al.  Moderating binge drinking: it is possible to change behaviour if you plan it in advance. , 1996, Alcohol and alcoholism.

[50]  J. Greenberg,et al.  Toward an integration of cognitive and motivational perspectives on social inference: A biased hypothesis-testing model , 1987 .