Simultaneous brightness and apparent depth from true colors on grey: Chevreul revisited.

We show that true colors as defined by Chevreul (1839) produce unsuspected simultaneous brightness induction effects on their immediate grey backgrounds when these are placed on a darker (black) general background surrounding two spatially separated configurations. Assimilation and apparent contrast may occur in one and the same stimulus display. We examined the possible link between these effects and the perceived depth of the color patterns which induce them as a function of their luminance contrast. Patterns of square-shaped inducers of a single color (red, green, blue, yellow, or grey) were placed on background fields of a lighter and a darker grey, presented on a darker screen. Inducers were always darker on one side of the display and brighter on the other in a given trial. The intensity of the grey backgrounds varied between trials only. This permitted generating four inducer luminance contrasts, presented in random order, for each color. Background fields were either spatially separated or consisted of a single grey field on the black screen. Experiments were run under three environmental conditions: dark-adaptation, daylight, and rod-saturation after exposure to bright light. In a first task, we measured probabilities of contrast, assimilation, and no effect in a three-alternative forced-choice procedure (background appears brighter on the 'left', on the 'right' or the 'same'). Visual adaptation and inducer contrast had no significant influence on the induction effects produced by colored inducers. Achromatic inducers produced significantly stronger contrast effects after dark-adaptation, and significantly stronger assimilation in daylight conditions. Grouping two backgrounds into a single one was found to significantly decrease probabilities of apparent contrast. Under the same conditions, we measured probabilities of the inducers to be perceived as nearer to the observer (inducers appear nearer on 'left', on 'right' or the 'same'). These, as predicted by Chevreul's law of contrast, were determined by the luminance contrast of the inducers only, with significantly higher probabilities of brighter inducers to be seen as nearer, and a marked asymmetry between effects produced by inducers of opposite sign. Implications of these findings for theories which attempt to link simultaneous induction effects to the relative depth of object surfaces in the visual field are discussed.

[1]  Jacob Beck,et al.  Contrast and assimilation in lightness judgments , 1966 .

[2]  H. Helson Studies of anomalous contrast and assimilation. , 1963, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[3]  P Simonet,et al.  Effect of illuminance on the directions of chromostereopsis and transverse chromatic aberration observed with natural pupils , 1990, Ophthalmic & physiological optics : the journal of the British College of Ophthalmic Opticians.

[4]  Stephen Grossberg,et al.  Depth perception from pairs of overlapping cues in pictorial displays. , 2002, Spatial vision.

[5]  L. Sharpe,et al.  Assimilative hue shifts in color depend on bar width , 1986 .

[6]  David Katz,et al.  Die Erscheinungsweisen der Farben und ihre Beeinflussung durch die individuelle Erfahrung , 1911 .

[7]  Baingio Pinna,et al.  The Organization of Shape and Color in Vision and Art , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[8]  Jiro Hamada,et al.  Asymmetric lightness cancellation in Craik-O'Brien patterns of negative and positive contrast , 1985, Biological Cybernetics.

[9]  S. Shevell,et al.  Color in complex scenes. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.

[10]  R. Blake,et al.  Interocular suppression differentially affects achromatic and chromatic mechanisms , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[11]  B. Stabell,et al.  The effect of rod acitvity on colour matching functions , 1975, Vision Research.

[12]  Birgitta Dresp,et al.  Long-range spatial integration across contrast signs: a probabilistic mechanism? , 2005, Vision Research.

[13]  D. Foster,et al.  Color constancy: Phenomenal or projective? , 2008, Perception & psychophysics.

[14]  Arash Yazdanbakhsh,et al.  Perceiving opponent hues in color induction displays. , 2011, Seeing and perceiving.

[15]  A. M. Rohaly,et al.  The effects of contrast on perceived depth and depth discrimination , 1999, Vision Research.

[16]  C. D. Weert The role of colours in the formation of subjective contours , 1983 .

[17]  Joel Pokorny,et al.  Rod contributions to color perception: Linear with rod contrast , 2007, Vision Research.

[18]  Eugène Chevreul,et al.  De la loi du contraste simultané des couleurs et de l'assortiment des objets colorés , 1839 .

[19]  Adam Reeves,et al.  Lighting, backlighting and watercolor illusions and the laws of figurality. , 2006, Spatial vision.

[20]  Birgitta Dresp,et al.  Local brightness mechanisms sketch out surfaces but do not fill them in: Psychophysical evidence in the Kanizsa square , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[21]  R. Desimone,et al.  Visual properties of neurons in area V4 of the macaque: sensitivity to stimulus form. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  S. Grossberg Cortical dynamics of three-dimensional figure-ground perception of two-dimensional pictures. , 1997, Psychological review.

[23]  John S Werner,et al.  Spatial profile of contours inducing long-range color assimilation , 2006, Visual Neuroscience.

[24]  Birgitta Dresp,et al.  Asymmetrical contrast effects induced by luminance and color configurations , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[25]  M Dengler,et al.  Color stereopsis: A model for depth reversals based on border contrast , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[26]  Dale Purves,et al.  Natural scene statistics as the universal basis of color context effects , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[27]  R Shapley,et al.  Contrast and assimilation in the perception of brightness. , 1985, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[28]  L. Sharpe,et al.  Assimilative hue shifts in color gratings depend on bar width. , 1986, Perception & psychophysics.

[29]  G. Sperling,et al.  Luminance controls the perceived 3-D structure of dynamic 2-D displays. , 1983 .

[30]  B. Pinna A new perceptual problem: The amodal completion of color , 2008, Visual Neuroscience.

[31]  S. Blackburn,et al.  Contrast as a depth cue , 1994, Vision Research.

[32]  S. Magnussen,et al.  Temporal frequency characteristics of spatial interaction in human vision , 1975, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  Rachel Pierson,et al.  Dissociation of color and figure-ground effects in the watercolor illusion. , 2010, Spatial vision.

[34]  L. Festinger,et al.  The effect of attention on brightness contrast and assimilation. , 1970, The American journal of psychology.

[35]  E. G. Heinemann,et al.  Simultaneous brightness induction as a function of inducing and test-field luminances. , 1955, Journal of experimental psychology.

[36]  H. Gerrits,et al.  Simultaneous contrast, filling-in process and information processing in man's visual system , 1970, Experimental Brain Research.

[37]  Traffic Signals and Depth Perception , 1967, Science.

[38]  Birgitta Dresp,et al.  Interaction of color and geometric cues in depth perception: When does “red” mean “near”? , 2004, Psychological research.

[39]  L. Spillmann,et al.  Assimilation: Asymmetry between brightness and darkness? , 1995, Vision Research.