Cheap talk and credibility: The consequences of confidence and accuracy on advisor credibility and persuasiveness Organizational Decision

Is it possible to increase one’s influence simply by behaving more confidently? Prior research presents two competing hypotheses: (1) the confidence heuristic holds that more confidence increases credibility, and (2) the calibration hypothesis asserts that overconfidence will backfire when others find out. Study 1 reveals that, consistent with the calibration hypothesis, while accurate advisors benefit from displaying confidence, confident but inaccurate advisors receive low credibility ratings. However, Study 2 shows that when feedback on advisor accuracy is unavailable or costly, confident advisors hold sway regardless of accuracy. People also made less effort to determine the accuracy of confident advisors; interest in buying advisor performance data decreased as the advisor’s confidence went up. These results add to our understanding of how advisor confidence, accuracy, and calibration influence others. (cid:2) 2013 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

[1]  B. Newell Judgment Under Uncertainty , 2013 .

[2]  G. Loewenstein,et al.  The burden of disclosure: increased compliance with distrusted advice. , 2013, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[3]  Cameron Anderson,et al.  A status-enhancement account of overconfidence. , 2012, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[4]  Elizabeth R. Tenney,et al.  Complex Social Consequences of Self-Knowledge , 2011 .

[5]  Van Swol,et al.  Forecasting another’s enjoyment versus giving the right answer: Trust, shared values, task effects, and confidence in improving the acceptance of advice , 2011 .

[6]  Lyn M. Van Swol,et al.  The Effects of Confidence and Advisor Motives on Advice Utilization , 2009, Commun. Res..

[7]  M. Banaji,et al.  PREDICTIVE VALIDITY OF THE IAT 1 RUNNING HEAD : PREDICTIVE VALIDITY OF THE IAT Understanding and Using the Implicit Association Test : III . Meta-analysis of Predictive Validity , 2006 .

[8]  C. Anderson,et al.  Why do dominant personalities attain influence in face-to-face groups? The competence-signaling effects of trait dominance. , 2009, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[9]  Robert J. MacCoun,et al.  The benefits of knowing what you know (and what you don’t): How calibration affects credibility , 2008 .

[10]  D. Moore,et al.  Use of absolute and comparative performance feedback in absolute and comparative judgments and decisions , 2008 .

[11]  D. Moore,et al.  The trouble with overconfidence. , 2008, Psychological review.

[12]  R. Baumeister,et al.  Toward a Physiology of Dual-Process Reasoning and Judgment: Lemonade, Willpower, and Expensive Rule-Based Analysis , 2008, Psychological science.

[13]  C. Ridgeway,et al.  Social Status and Group Structure , 2008 .

[14]  James Price Dillard,et al.  The Relationship Between the Perceived and Actual Effectiveness of Persuasive Messages: A Meta-Analysis With Implications for Formative Campaign Research , 2007 .

[15]  Reid Hastie,et al.  Calibration Trumps Confidence as a Basis for Witness Credibility , 2006, Psychological science.

[16]  D. Moore,et al.  Effects of Task Difficulty on Use of Advice , 2007 .

[17]  S. Bonaccio,et al.  Advice taking and decision-making: An integrative literature review, and implications for the organizational sciences , 2006 .

[18]  C. Ridgeway,et al.  Consensus and the Creation of Status Beliefs , 2006 .

[19]  Neil Brewer,et al.  The confidence-accuracy relationship in eyewitness identification: effects of lineup instructions, foil similarity, and target-absent base rates. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[20]  Lyn M Van Swol,et al.  Factors affecting the acceptance of expert advice. , 2005, The British journal of social psychology.

[21]  Ilan Yaniv,et al.  Receiving Other People's Advice: Influence and Benefit , 2004 .

[22]  Eric R. Stone,et al.  Intuitive evaluation of likelihood judgment producers: evidence for a confidence heuristic , 2004 .

[23]  Shu Li,et al.  Respondents in Asian Cultures (e.g., Chinese) are More Risk-Seeking and More Overconfident than Respondents in Other Cultures (e.g., in United States) but the Reciprocal Predictions are in Total Opposition: How and Why? , 2004 .

[24]  S. Kassin Eyewitness Identification: Retrospective Self-Awareness and the Accuracy-Confidence Correlation , 1985 .

[25]  Jonathan Evans In two minds: dual-process accounts of reasoning , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[26]  D. Kahneman A perspective on judgment and choice: mapping bounded rationality. , 2003, The American psychologist.

[27]  Anne Burke,et al.  Effects of Testimonial Inconsistencies and Eyewitness Confidence on Mock-Juror Judgments , 2002, Law and human behavior.

[28]  Janet A. Sniezek,et al.  Trust, Confidence, and Expertise in a Judge-Advisor System. , 2001, Organizational behavior and human decision processes.

[29]  K. Teigen,et al.  Why is p = .90 better than p = .70? Preference for definitive predictions by lay consumers of probability judgements. , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[30]  Mary Lundeberg,et al.  Cultural influences on confidence : Country and gender , 2000 .

[31]  Jack B. Soll,et al.  Overconfidence: It Depends on How, What, and Whom You Ask. , 1999, Organizational behavior and human decision processes.

[32]  Baruch Fischhoff,et al.  Fifty–fifty = 50%? , 1999 .

[33]  Phillips,et al.  Antecedents of Leader Utilization of Staff Input in Decision-Making Teams. , 1999, Organizational behavior and human decision processes.

[34]  Sniezek,et al.  The Social Influence of Confidence in Group Decision Making , 1997, Journal of experimental social psychology.

[35]  Ilan Yaniv ORGANIZATIONAL BEHAVIOR AND HUMAN DECISION PROCESSES, VOLUME 69, ISSUE 3 , 1997 .

[36]  John S. Shaw,et al.  Repeated postevent questioning can lead to elevated levels of eyewitness confidence , 1996 .

[37]  N. Kerr,et al.  Bias in judgment: Comparing individuals and groups. , 1996 .

[38]  Garrett L. Berman,et al.  Effects of inconsistencies in eyewitness testimony on mock-juror decision-making , 1996 .

[39]  James Ramirez,et al.  Good probabilistic forecasters: The ‘consumer's’ perspective , 1996 .

[40]  Janet A. Sniezek,et al.  Cueing and Cognitive Conflict in Judge-Advisor Decision Making , 1995 .

[41]  G. Wells,et al.  What do we know about eyewitness identification? , 1993, The American psychologist.

[42]  Janet A. Sniezek,et al.  Groups under uncertainty: An examination of confidence in group decision making☆ , 1992 .

[43]  P. Ekman,et al.  Who can catch a liar? , 1991, The American psychologist.

[44]  Eric J. Johnson,et al.  The Adaptive Decision-Maker : Effort and Accuracy in Choice , 2022 .

[45]  P. R. Laughlin,et al.  Demonstrability and social combination processes on mathematical intellective tasks. , 1986 .

[46]  Terence R. Mitchell,et al.  A cost-benefit mechanism for selecting problem-solving strategies: Some extensions and empirical tests , 1982 .

[47]  I. Ajzen,et al.  Understanding Attitudes and Predicting Social Behavior , 1980 .

[48]  R. Lindsay,et al.  Accuracy, confidence, and juror perceptions in eyewitness identification. , 1979, The Journal of applied psychology.

[49]  I. Ajzen,et al.  Attitude-behavior relations: A theoretical analysis and review of empirical research. , 1977 .

[50]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. , 1977 .

[51]  S. Chaiken,et al.  Communication modality as a determinant of message persuasiveness and message comprehensibility. , 1976 .

[52]  A. Tversky,et al.  Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases , 1974, Science.