The influence of anticipated counterfactual regret on behavior.

Three experiments tested the hypothesis that the anticipation of counterfactual regret influences decision making and behavior. This hypothesis was examined under conditions of uncertainty in which the behavioral alternatives were equally desirable (or undesirable) and there was no clear default alternative. In Experiment 1, participants read a scenario in which the salience of anticipated counterfactual regret associated with two behavioral options was manipulated. Their behavioral intentions demonstrated avoidance of the option associated with salient counterfactual regret. Experiment 2 examined behavior in a gaming situation in which participants chose whether or not to purchase insurance to protect their treasure. Participants who anticipated counterfactual regret made insurance purchase decisions in a way that minimized the chances of experiencing this regret. To rule out several alternative explanations for the results of the first two experiments, the content of the anticipated counterfactual and the salience of anticipated regret were orthogonally manipulated in a third experiment. As expected, the influence of the anticipated counterfactual on insurance-buying behavior was significantly stronger when the associated regret was salient than when it was not. These findings are discussed in terms of both their positive and negative implications for the self in decision-making contexts. © 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

[1]  K. Markman,et al.  The Mental Simulation of Better and Worse Possible Worlds , 1993 .

[2]  B. Weiner A Cognitive (Attribution)-Emotion-Action Model of Motivated Behavior: An Analysis of Judgments of Help-Giving. , 1980 .

[3]  David E. Bell,et al.  Regret in Decision Making under Uncertainty , 1982, Oper. Res..

[4]  Joop van der Pligt,et al.  Anticipated regret and time perspective: Changing sexual risk-taking behavior. , 1996 .

[5]  R. W. Rogers,et al.  Protection motivation and self-efficacy: A revised theory of fear appeals and attitude change , 1983 .

[6]  A. Tversky,et al.  The Psychology of Preferences , 1982 .

[7]  D. Boninger,et al.  Counterfactual thinking: From what might have been to what may be. , 1994 .

[8]  R. Sugden,et al.  Regret Theory: An alternative theory of rational choice under uncertainty Review of Economic Studies , 1982 .

[9]  Jonathan Baron,et al.  Behavioral Law and Economics: Reluctance to Vaccinate: Omission Bias and Ambiguity , 1990 .

[10]  H. Leventhal,et al.  Findings and Theory in the Study of Fear Communications , 1970 .

[11]  J. Landman Regret and Elation Following Action and Inaction , 1987 .

[12]  T. Gilovich,et al.  The temporal pattern to the experience of regret. , 1994, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[13]  Joan Meyers-Levy,et al.  When Timing Matters: The Influence of Temporal Distance on Consumers' Affective and Persuasive Responses , 1992 .

[14]  N. Roese The Functional Basis of Counterfactual Thinking , 1994 .

[15]  H Kunreuther,et al.  Omission Bias and Pertussis Vaccination , 1994, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[16]  J. Landman,et al.  Regret: The Persistence of the Possible , 1993 .

[17]  T. Gilovich,et al.  The experience of regret: what, when, and why. , 1995, Psychological review.

[18]  Thane S. Pittman,et al.  Inaction Inertia: Foregoing Future Benefits as a Result of an Initial Failure to Act , 1995 .

[19]  David E. Bell,et al.  Risk Premiums for Decision Regret , 1983 .

[20]  Dale T. Miller,et al.  Counterfactual Thinking and Victim Compensation , 1986 .

[21]  Thane S. Pittman,et al.  The consequences of doing nothing: Inaction inertia as avoidance of anticipated counterfactual regret. , 1998 .

[22]  Steven J. Sherman,et al.  The Role of Counterfactual Thinking in Judgments of Affect , 1990 .

[23]  M. Zeelenberg,et al.  Consequences of Regret Aversion: Effects of Expected Feedback on Risky Decision Making , 1996 .

[24]  M. Bar-Hillel,et al.  Why are people reluctant to exchange lottery tickets , 1996 .

[25]  Philip E. Tetlock,et al.  Accountability amplifies the status quo effect when change creates victims , 1994 .

[26]  J. van der Pligt,et al.  Anticipated Affect and Behavioral Choice , 1996 .

[27]  M. Zeelenberg,et al.  Consequences of regret aversion: 2. Additional evidence for effects of feedback on decision making , 1997 .

[28]  G. Wells,et al.  Mental Simulation of Causality , 1989 .

[29]  J van der Pligt,et al.  Anticipated affective reactions and prevention of AIDS. , 1995, The British journal of social psychology.

[30]  A. Tversky,et al.  On the psychology of prediction , 1973 .

[31]  L. Sanna,et al.  Antecedents to Spontaneous Counterfactual Thinking: Effects of Expectancy Violation and Outcome Valence , 1996 .

[32]  Antony Stephen Reid Manstead,et al.  The moderating role of self-efficacy beliefs in the relationship between anticipated feelings of regret and condom use , 1997 .

[33]  Richard P. Larrick,et al.  Avoiding Regret in Decisions with Feedback: A Negotiation Example , 1995 .

[34]  Dale T. Miller,et al.  Norm theory: Comparing reality to its alternatives , 1986 .

[35]  I. Simonson,et al.  THE INFLUENCE OF ANTICIPATING REGRET AND RESPONSIBILITY ON PURCHASE DECISIONS , 1992 .

[36]  Thomas Gilovich,et al.  Commission, Omission, and Dissonance Reduction: Coping with Regret in the "Monty Hall" Problem , 1995 .