A right visual field advantage for visual processing of manipulable objects
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Tim Shallice,et al. The different neural correlates of action and functional knowledge in semantic memory: an FMRI study. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.
[2] A. Chatterjee. Disembodying cognition , 2010, Language and Cognition.
[3] Sergio E. Chaigneau,et al. THE SIMILARITY-IN-TOPOGRAPHY PRINCIPLE: RECONCILING THEORIES OF CONCEPTUAL DEFICITS , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[4] L. Jakobson,et al. A neurological dissociation between perceiving objects and grasping them , 1991, Nature.
[5] E. Ringelstein,et al. Handedness and hemispheric language dominance in healthy humans. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[6] Bradford Z. Mahon,et al. Action-Related Properties Shape Object Representations in the Ventral Stream , 2007, Neuron.
[7] Gregory Króliczak,et al. Atypical lateralization of language predicts cerebral asymmetries in parietal gesture representations , 2011, Neuropsychologia.
[8] M. Brett,et al. Actions Speak Louder Than Functions: The Importance of Manipulability and Action in Tool Representation , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[9] D. Schacter,et al. Perceptual specificity in visual object priming: functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for a laterality difference in fusiform cortex , 2001, Neuropsychologia.
[10] Leslie G. Ungerleider. Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .
[11] Salvatore Maria Aglioti,et al. The Sound of Actions in Apraxia , 2008, Current Biology.
[12] Alfonso Caramazza,et al. The orchestration of the sensory-motor systems: Clues from Neuropsychology , 2005, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[13] Bradford Z. Mahon,et al. Action-related properties of objects shape object representations in the ventral stream , 2007 .
[14] J R Hodges,et al. "What" and "how": evidence for the dissociation of object knowledge and mechanical problem-solving skills in the human brain. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[15] C. Koch,et al. A category-specific response to animals in the right human amygdala , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.
[16] Daniel N. Bub,et al. Different modes of word recognition in the left and right visual fields , 1988, Brain and Language.
[17] D. J. Felleman,et al. Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. , 1991, Cerebral cortex.
[18] L. Buxbaum,et al. Distinctions between manipulation and function knowledge of objects: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[19] S. Thorpe,et al. Speed of processing in the human visual system , 1996, Nature.
[20] R. Ilmoniemi,et al. Functional links between motor and language systems , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.
[21] Scott T. Grafton,et al. A distributed left hemisphere network active during planning of everyday tool use skills. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.
[22] Alfonso Caramazza,et al. What is the role of motor simulation in action and object recognition? Evidence from apraxia , 2007, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[23] M. Kiefer,et al. Action observation can prime visual object recognition , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.
[24] K. Heilman,et al. A Cognitive Neuropsychological Model of Limb Praxis , 1991 .
[25] Leif D. Nelson,et al. False-Positive Psychology , 2011, Psychological science.
[26] F. Fang,et al. Cortical responses to invisible objects in the human dorsal and ventral pathways , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[27] Bradford Z. Mahon,et al. The Role of the Dorsal Visual Processing Stream in Tool Identification , 2010, Psychological science.
[28] Melvyn A Goodale,et al. Dual routes to action: contributions of the dorsal and ventral streams to adaptive behavior. , 2005, Progress in brain research.
[29] Ronald A. Rensink,et al. Competition for consciousness among visual events: the psychophysics of reentrant visual processes. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[30] Alex Martin,et al. Representation of Manipulable Man-Made Objects in the Dorsal Stream , 2000, NeuroImage.
[31] M. Goodale,et al. Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.
[32] L. Barsalou. Grounded cognition. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.
[33] Alex Martin,et al. NEURAL FOUNDATIONS FOR UNDERSTANDING SOCIAL AND MECHANICAL CONCEPTS , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[34] R. Mansfield,et al. Analysis of visual behavior , 1982 .
[35] Scott T. Grafton,et al. Graspable objects grab attention when the potential for action is recognized , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[36] E. Thorndike. Ideo-motor action. , 1913 .
[37] F. Previc. Functional specialization in the lower and upper visual fields in humans: Its ecological origins and neurophysiological implications , 1990, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[38] A. Caramazza,et al. Letter identification processes in reading: Distractor interference reveals an automatically engaged, domain-specific mechanism , 2006, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[39] G. Glover,et al. Retinotopic organization in human visual cortex and the spatial precision of functional MRI. , 1997, Cerebral cortex.
[40] Alfonso Caramazza,et al. All Talk and No Action: A Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Study of Motor Cortex Activation during Action Word Production , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[41] B. Mesquita,et al. Adjustment to Chronic Diseases and Terminal Illness Health Psychology : Psychological Adjustment to Chronic Disease , 2006 .
[42] A. Sirigu,et al. The role of sensorimotor experience in object recognition. A case of multimodal agnosia. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[43] JamesW. Lewis. Cortical Networks Related to Human Use of Tools , 2006, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.
[44] K. Heilman,et al. Ideational apraxia: A deficit in tool selection and use , 1989, Annals of neurology.
[45] Agnes Roby-Brami,et al. A neuropsychological perspective on the link between language and praxis in modern humans , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[46] G. Lakoff,et al. The Brain's concepts: the role of the Sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge , 2005, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[47] Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al. Brain mechanisms linking language and action , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[48] M. Kiefer,et al. Conceptual representations in mind and brain: Theoretical developments, current evidence and future directions , 2012, Cortex.
[49] J. Haxby,et al. Attribute-based neural substrates in temporal cortex for perceiving and knowing about objects , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.
[50] Markus Graf,et al. The role of action representations in visual object recognition , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.
[51] M S Gazzaniga,et al. Dissociation of language and cognition. A psychological profile of two disconnected right hemispheres. , 1984, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[52] Alfonso Caramazza,et al. Differential Activity for Animals and Manipulable Objects in the Anterior Temporal Lobes , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[53] C. Koch,et al. Visual Selective Behavior Can Be Triggered by a Feed-Forward Process , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[54] A. Damasio,et al. A neural basis for the retrieval of conceptual knowledge , 1997, Neuropsychologia.
[55] M. Brysbaert,et al. A right visual field advantage for tool-recognition in the visual half-field paradigm , 2011, Neuropsychologia.
[56] Gregory Króliczak,et al. A common network in the left cerebral hemisphere represents planning of tool use pantomimes and familiar intransitive gestures at the hand-independent level. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.
[57] Karl J. Friston,et al. Two distinct neural mechanisms for category-selective responses. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.
[58] Zoë R. Hunter,et al. Visual half-field experiments are a good measure of cerebral language dominance if used properly: Evidence from fMRI , 2008, Neuropsychologia.
[59] C. Chiarello,et al. Lexical decision and naming asymmetries: Influence of response selection and response bias , 1988, Brain and Language.
[60] Alfonso Caramazza,et al. Unconscious processing dissociates along categorical lines , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[61] Richard J. Epstein,et al. Current perspectives in dysphasia , 1985 .
[62] M. Wallace,et al. Visual Localization Ability Influences Cross-Modal Bias , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[63] D. Kemmerer,et al. The Two-Level Theory of verb meaning: An approach to integrating the semantics of action with the mirror neuron system , 2010, Brain and Language.
[64] Scott H. Johnson-Frey. The neural bases of complex tool use in humans , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[65] G. Gigli,et al. Degraded Semantic Knowledge And Accurate Object Use , 2007, Cortex.
[66] S. Kastner,et al. Two hierarchically organized neural systems for object information in human visual cortex , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.
[67] Leslie G. Ungerleider. Functional Brain Imaging Studies of Cortical Mechanisms for Memory , 1995, Science.
[68] H. Heuer,et al. Perspectives on Perception and Action , 1989 .
[69] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.
[70] I. Johnsrude,et al. Somatotopic Representation of Action Words in Human Motor and Premotor Cortex , 2004, Neuron.
[71] J. Grainger,et al. Effects of prime—target spatial separation and attentional deployment on masked repetition priming , 2008, Perception & psychophysics.
[72] Bradford Z. Mahon,et al. A critical look at the embodied cognition hypothesis and a new proposal for grounding conceptual content , 2008, Journal of Physiology-Paris.