The accuracy of pain and fatigue items across different reporting periods

Abstract The length of the reporting period specified for items assessing pain and fatigue varies among instruments. How the length of recall impacts the accuracy of symptom reporting is largely unknown. This study investigated the accuracy of ratings for reporting periods ranging from 1 day to 28 days for several items from widely used pain and fatigue measures (SF36v2, Brief Pain Inventory, McGill Pain Questionnaire, Brief Fatigue Inventory). Patients from a community rheumatology practice (N = 83) completed momentary pain and fatigue items on average of 5.4 times per day for a month using an electronic diary. Averaged momentary ratings formed the basis for comparison with recall ratings interspersed throughout the month referencing 1‐day, 3‐day, 7‐day, and 28‐day periods. As found in previous research, recall ratings were consistently inflated relative to averaged momentary ratings. Across most items, 1‐day recall corresponded well to the averaged momentary assessments for the day. Several, but not all, items demonstrated substantial correlations across the different reporting periods. An additional 7 day‐by‐day recall task suggested that patients have increasing difficulty actually remembering symptom levels beyond the past several days. These data were collected while patients were receiving usual care and may not generalize to conditions where new interventions are being introduced and outcomes evaluated. Reporting periods can influence the accuracy of retrospective symptom reports and should be a consideration in study design.

[1]  R. Melzack The McGill Pain Questionnaire: Major properties and scoring methods , 1975, PAIN.

[2]  C. Cleeland,et al.  Development of the Wisconsin Brief Pain Questionnaire to assess pain in cancer and other diseases , 1983, Pain.

[3]  C. Cleeland,et al.  Fatigue and sleep disturbance in patients with cancer, patients with clinical depression, and community-dwelling adults. , 2003, Journal of pain and symptom management.

[4]  C. Cleeland,et al.  Validity of the Brief Pain Inventory for Use in Documenting the Outcomes of Patients With Noncancer Pain , 2004, The Clinical journal of pain.

[5]  D. Cella,et al.  Measuring fatigue and other anemia-related symptoms with the Functional Assessment of Cancer Therapy (FACT) measurement system. , 1997, Journal of pain and symptom management.

[6]  John L. Reeves,et al.  Memory for pain: Relation between past and present pain intensity , 1985, Pain.

[7]  L. Mariani,et al.  Pain measurement in cancer patients: a comparison of six methods , 1994, Pain.

[8]  C. Cleeland,et al.  The prevalence and severity of pain in cancer , 1982, Cancer.

[9]  C. Cleeland,et al.  The rapid assessment of fatigue severity in cancer patients , 1999, Cancer.

[10]  Michael D. Robinson,et al.  Belief and feeling: evidence for an accessibility model of emotional self-report. , 2002, Psychological bulletin.

[11]  Barbara Gandek,et al.  How to score version 2 of the SF-12(R) health survey (with a supplement documenting version 1) , 2005 .

[12]  Michael D. Robinson,et al.  Episodic and semantic knowledge in emotional self-report: evidence for two judgment processes. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[13]  A. Stone,et al.  Recalled pain ratings: a complex and poorly defined task. , 2006, The journal of pain : official journal of the American Pain Society.

[14]  M. Ross Relation of Implicit Theories to the Construction of Personal Histories , 1989 .

[15]  A. Stone,et al.  A systematic review of measures used to assess chronic musculoskeletal pain in clinical and randomized controlled clinical trials. , 2007, The journal of pain : official journal of the American Pain Society.

[16]  Ronald Melzack,et al.  The short-form McGill pain questionnaire , 1987, Pain.

[17]  C. Cleeland,et al.  Pain assessment: global use of the Brief Pain Inventory. , 1994, Annals of the Academy of Medicine, Singapore.

[18]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Patients' memories of painful medical treatments: real-time and retrospective evaluations of two minimally invasive procedures , 1996, Pain.

[19]  Joseph E. Schwartz,et al.  Variability of Momentary Pain Predicts Recall of Weekly Pain: A Consequence of the Peak (or Salience) Memory Heuristic , 2005, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[20]  B. Fredrickson Extracting meaning from past affective experiences: The importance of peaks, ends, and specific emotions , 2000 .

[21]  S. Shiffman,et al.  Understanding recall of weekly pain from a momentary assessment perspective: absolute agreement, between- and within-person consistency, and judged change in weekly pain , 2004, Pain.

[22]  P. Salovey,et al.  The accuracy of memory for pain , 1993 .

[23]  I. McDowell,et al.  Measuring health: A guide to rating scales and questionnaires, 3rd ed. , 2006 .

[24]  T. Revenson,et al.  Handbook of Health Psychology , 2001 .

[25]  Barbara Gandek,et al.  Sf-12v2 How to score version 2 of the sf-12 health survey. Quality Metric Incorporated , 2002 .

[26]  D. Osoba Effect of Cancer On Quality of Life , 1991 .

[27]  A. Stone,et al.  Context Effects in Survey Ratings of Health, Symptoms, and Satisfaction , 2008, Medical care.

[28]  Ronald Melzack,et al.  Pain Measurement and Assessment , 1983 .

[29]  A. Stone,et al.  Does the peak-end phenomenon observed in laboratory pain studies apply to real-world pain in rheumatoid arthritics? , 2000, The journal of pain : official journal of the American Pain Society.

[30]  A. Damiano,et al.  Comparison of responses to SF-36 Health Survey questions with one-week and four-week recall periods. , 1997, Health services research.

[31]  T. Mills,et al.  Measuring Health: A Guide to Rating Scales and Questionnaires , 2006 .

[32]  Daniel Kahneman,et al.  Memories of colonoscopy: a randomized trial , 2003, Pain.

[33]  Ronald Melzack,et al.  The McGill Pain Questionnaire: Appraisal and current status. , 2001 .

[34]  D. Cella Effect of Cancer on Quality of Life , 1992, Quality of Life Research.