Categorization and feature specification in phonological acquisition

Distinctive feature specification and representation in phonological acquisition are examined in the context of underspecification theory. Subjects were 30 children aged (3;1 to 5;10) who exhibited systematic differences in their linguistic knowledge of target phonological contrasts. A free classification task was used to tap children's conceptual knowledge of these contrasts, with features of place and manner experimentally manipulated. Three questions were addressed: which features do children use to categorize segmental information, do the defining features of a category shift as the phonological system advances, and which framework of underspecification theory best accounts for the results? All children categorized segments on the basis of marked nonredundant featural properties, and used only one feature value to define category membership consistent with radical underspecification. Linguistic knowledge and linguistic input both influenced children's category judgements, but to different degrees. The emergence of phonological categories involved increasing feature differentiation as the child's productive phonology advanced.

[1]  Judith A. Gierut On the relationship between phonological knowledge and generalization learning in misarticulating children , 1986 .

[2]  A S House,et al.  Phonological oppositions in children: a perceptual study. , 1971, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[3]  D. Dinnsen,et al.  A functional analysis of phonological knowledge and generalization learning in misarticulating children. , 1987, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[4]  J. McCarthy Feature Geometry and Dependency: A Review , 1988 .

[5]  W. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  The mental representation of lexical form: A phonological approach to the recognition lexicon , 1991, Cognition.

[6]  D. Dinnsen,et al.  Context-sensitive underspecification and the acquisition of phonemic contrasts , 1996, Journal of Child Language.

[7]  D. Sherman,et al.  Review of Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation. , 1970 .

[8]  R. Treiman,et al.  Common phoneme and overall similarity relations among spoken syllables: Their use by children and adults , 1982, Journal of psycholinguistic research.

[9]  Keren Rice,et al.  Segmental complexity and the structure of inventories , 1993 .

[10]  G. E. MacKinnon,et al.  Reading Research Advances in Theory and Practice , 1985 .

[11]  S. Carey Conceptual Change in Childhood , 1985 .

[12]  S. Velleman The role of linguistic perception in later phonological development , 1988, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[13]  P. Ladefoged A course in phonetics , 1975 .

[14]  Michael Kenstowicz,et al.  Phonology In Generative Grammar , 1994 .

[15]  J. Gierut,et al.  Phonemic structures of delayed phonological systems , 1994, Journal of Child Language.

[16]  Junko Ito,et al.  Feature Predictability and Underspecification: Palatal Prosody in Japanese Mimetics , 1989 .

[17]  Diana Archangeli,et al.  Aspects of underspecification theory , 1988, Phonology.

[18]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  The role of phonemes and syllables in the perceived similarity of speech sounds for children , 1986, Memory & cognition.

[19]  L. Menn,et al.  Phonological development : models, research, implications , 1994 .

[20]  S. B. Chin,et al.  Feature geometry in disordered phonologies , 1991 .

[21]  S. B. Chin,et al.  Some constraints on functionally disordered phonologies: phonetic inventories and phonotactics. , 1990, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[22]  R. Aslin,et al.  Three- and four-year-olds’ perceptual confusions for spoken words , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[23]  J. Archibald Phonological Acquisition and Phonological Theory , 1996 .

[24]  A survey of phonological features , 1988 .

[25]  A. B. Smit,et al.  The Iowa Articulation Norms Project and Its Nebraska Replication , 1990 .