Position sense at the human forearm in the horizontal plane during loading and vibration of elbow muscles

When blindfolded subjects match the position of their forearms in the vertical plane they rely on signals coming from the periphery as well as from the central motor command. The command signal provides a positional cue from the accompanying effort sensation required to hold the arm against gravity. Here we have asked, does a centrally generated effort signal contribute to position sense in the horizontal plane, where gravity cannot play a role? Blindfolded subjects were required to match forearm position for the unloaded arm and when flexors or extensors were bearing 10%, 25% or 40% of maximum loads. Before each match the reference arm was conditioned by contracting elbow muscles while the arm was held flexed or extended. For the unloaded arm conditioning led to a consistent pattern of errors which was attributed to signals from flexor and extensor muscle spindles. When elbow muscles were loaded the errors from conditioning converged, presumably because the spindles had become coactivated through the fusimotor system during the load‐bearing contraction. However, this convergence was seen only when subjects supported a static load. When they moved the load differences in errors from conditioning persisted. Muscle vibration during load bearing or moving a load did not alter the distribution of errors. It is concluded that for position sense of an unloaded arm in the horizontal plane the brain relies on signals from muscle spindles. When the arm is loaded, an additional signal of central origin contributes, but only if the load is moved.

[1]  J. C. Gilhodes,et al.  Perceptual and motor effects of agonist-antagonist muscle vibration in man , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  U. Proske,et al.  Effect of muscle fatigue on the sense of limb position and movement , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  L R Young,et al.  Spatial orientation and posture during and following weightlessness: human experiments on Spacelab Life Sciences 1. , 1993, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[4]  A. Donnelly,et al.  Neuromuscular dysfunction following eccentric exercise. , 1995, Medicine and science in sports and exercise.

[5]  U. Proske,et al.  Human forearm position sense after fatigue of elbow flexor muscles , 2004, The Journal of physiology.

[6]  S. Gandevia,et al.  Cutaneous receptors contribute to kinesthesia at the index finger, elbow, and knee. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  D. McCloskey,et al.  The contribution of muscle afferents to kinaesthesia shown by vibration induced illusions of movement and by the effects of paralysing joint afferents. , 1972, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[8]  D. McCloskey,et al.  The role of joint receptors in human kinaesthesia when intramuscular receptors cannot contribute. , 1987, The Journal of physiology.

[9]  J. Lackner,et al.  Aspects of body self-calibration , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[10]  J. Roll,et al.  Alteration of proprioceptive messages induced by tendon vibration in man: a microneurographic study , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  J R Lackner,et al.  Illusions of postural, visual, and aircraft motion elicited by deep knee in the increased gravitoinertial force phase of parabolic flight. Evidence for dynamic sensory-motor calibration to earth gravity force levels. , 1981, Experimental brain research.

[12]  J. P. Nafe The sense of feeling. , 1929 .

[13]  William Noyas The Muscular Sense , 1887 .

[14]  E. Holst,et al.  Das Reafferenzprinzip , 2004, Naturwissenschaften.

[15]  Daniel M Wolpert,et al.  Common Encoding of Novel Dynamic Loads Applied to the Hand and Arm , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  ARTHUR LYNCH,et al.  The Muscular Sense. , 1930, Nature.

[17]  A. Vallbo,et al.  Human muscle spindle discharge during isometric voluntary contractions. Amplitude relations between spindle frequency and torque. , 1974, Acta physiologica Scandinavica.

[18]  J A Winter,et al.  Muscle spindle signals combine with the sense of effort to indicate limb position , 2005, The Journal of physiology.

[19]  D. McCloskey Differences between the senses of movement and position shown by the effects of loading and vibration of muscles in man. , 1973, Brain research.

[20]  E. Ribot-Ciscar,et al.  Ago-antagonist muscle spindle inputs contribute together to joint movement coding in man , 1998, Brain Research.

[21]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Two Eyes for an Eye: The Neuroscience of Force Escalation , 2003, Science.

[22]  J. Lackner,et al.  Illusions of postural, visual, and aircraft motion elicited by deep knee bends in the increased gravitoinertial force phase of parabolic flight , 1981, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  S C Gandevia,et al.  Effect of eccentric exercise on position sense at the human forearm in different postures. , 2006, Journal of applied physiology.

[24]  U. Proske,et al.  Thixotropy in skeletal muscle and in muscle spindles: A review , 1993, Progress in Neurobiology.

[25]  Responses of muscle spindles depend on their history of activation and movement. , 1988, Progress in brain research.

[26]  D. McCloskey,et al.  Muscle sense and effort: motor commands and judgments about muscular contractions. , 1983, Advances in neurology.

[27]  Hermann von Helmholtz,et al.  Treatise on Physiological Optics , 1962 .

[28]  U. Proske,et al.  A comparison of the effects of concentric versus eccentric exercise on force and position sense at the human elbow joint , 1997, Brain Research.

[29]  S. Gandevia,et al.  Motor commands contribute to human position sense , 2006, The Journal of physiology.