Meditation experiences evolve along a spectrum, ranging from an effortful struggle with the technique to deep transpersonal states where all dualities dissolve. The present study investigated to what extent the depth of meditation is influenced by the amount of meditation practice and the personality trait of absorption, and whether deep experiences influence the mindfulness of meditators in everyday life. A set of questionnaires (Meditation Depth Questionnaire, Tellegen Absorption Scale, and Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory) was distributed to meditators (N1⁄4 251) practicing different techniques. A structural equation modeling analysis revealed that absorption exerted a stronger influence on meditation depth (path coefficient: .48) than the amount of meditation practice (path coefficient: .21). Mindfulness was strongly influenced by meditation depth (path coefficient: .42) and moderately by absorption (path coefficient: .21). These complex relations between practice, personality, meditation experiences, and everyday behavior should be considered in future research on transpersonal states induced by meditation. Insight into our true nature is an ultimate goal of all spiritual traditions. In many mystical traditions, meditation practice is one primary approach to reach transpersonal states of non-duality denoted with a variety of terms (unio mystica, samadhi, nirvana, satori, transcendental consciousness). During the deepest states of meditation, profound changes in the perception of reality and the self occur (Gifford-May & Thompson, 1994). Irrespective of the meditation technique, advanced practitioners report rather similar experiences, which can be arranged along a dimension of meditation depth (Piron, 2001). This dimension is conceived as a spectrum ranging from an effortful struggle with the requirements of the chosen technique to the realization of the fundamental ground of all being, where all dualities dissolve (Piron, 2001). In the current study on meditation experiences, the concept of meditation depth is used as the key component, because it takes into account the differences in experiences between individuals and can be assessed quantitatively. From the perspective of meditation research the question as to which factors determine the progress of meditation is intriguing. Traditional teachings emphasize the importance of regular practice, e.g. the yoga sutras of Patanjali (sutra I.14; Vivekananda, 2001). However, the amount of practice required to reach deep meditations also depends on the inclination and openness towards mystical states, i.e., it is a matter of personality. In this respect, the personality trait of absorption is highly relevant, because it includes the openness for mystical states (Tellegen & Atkinson, email: Britta.K.Hoelzel@psychol.uni-giessen.de Copyright 2006 Transpersonal Institute The Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 2006, Vol. 38, No. 2 179 1974) and the meditative skill to focus on an object without being disturbed (Smith, 1987), which is a key requirement for successful meditation. A further interesting question concerning deep meditation experiences is their significance for everyday life. While an extensive body of literature exists on meditation and its effects (Murphy & Donovan, 1997; Shapiro & Walsh, 2003), the impact of deep experiences on everyday life has been rarely investigated. What are the consequences of transpersonal experiences during deep meditation? Are they only passing events or do they lead to sustained alterations of the mindset? Empirical research on these questions is scarce but it can be assumed that such experiences should influence a person’s behavior and perspective on life. Anecdotal reports describe a variety of effects such as heightened awareness and less automatic behavior during daily routines, acceptance and appreciation of one’s life, as well as a detached observer position that reduces reactions to distressing events and suffering in general (Hetherington, 2003; Kapleau Roshi, 1989). Mindfulness is a key concept that comprises all these aspects and is increasingly considered relevant for clinical treatment modalities (Baer, 2003; Germer, Siegel, & Fulton, 2005). A growing number of people practice meditation and it is successfully applied for prevention and therapy of different psychological disorders (Grossman, Niemann, Schmidt, & Walach, 2004), but basic research is still at its beginning. Although literature on meditation research comprises far more than a thousand publications (Murphy & Donovan, 1997), complex interactions between variables such as the personality of practitioners, the regularity of meditation practice, deep meditation experiences and their effects are not yet sufficiently understood. Here, we present a model that proposes specific relationships between these variables. The model is tested with empirical data of participants with different extents of meditation practice. Questionnaires that measure these specific variables were administered to a large sample of meditators in yoga centers. As we intended to investigate the relationship between latent constructs, the statistical method that is most appropriate is structural equation modeling (SEM). In the next section, the different components of the model (latent constructs)—namely meditation depth, absorption trait, meditation practice, and mindfulness—as well as the expected relations between them will be introduced. We will then introduce the structural model that specifies these relations and tests the fit of the empirical data with the postulated model. Furthermore, the postulated restricted model will be extended by allowing additional relations between the variables. Thus, the fit of two alternative models can be compared. DIFFERENT COMPONENTS OF THE MODEL
[1]
Marina Haase,et al.
Meditation
,
2004,
EuroPLoP.
[2]
H. Webster.
Correcting personality scales for response sets or suppression effects.
,
1958,
Psychological bulletin.
[3]
The EEG and the Depth of Meditation by
,
2008
.
[4]
S. West,et al.
The robustness of test statistics to nonnormality and specification error in confirmatory factor analysis.
,
1996
.
[5]
Stefan Schmidt,et al.
Measuring mindfulness—the Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory (FMI)
,
2006
.
[6]
Diana D. Suhr.
The Basics of Structural Equation Modeling
,
2006
.
[7]
D. Campbell,et al.
Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix.
,
1959,
Psychological bulletin.
[8]
M. Browne,et al.
Alternative Ways of Assessing Model Fit
,
1992
.
[9]
W. James.
Varieties of Religious Experience
,
2008
.
[10]
A. Deikman.
De-automatization and the Mystic Experience †.
,
1966,
Psychiatry.
[11]
Vishnu-Devananda.
Meditation and mantras
,
1978
.
[12]
Dietrich Lehmann,et al.
Psychobiology of altered states of consciousness.
,
2013,
Psychological bulletin.
[13]
Z. Segal,et al.
Mindfulness: A Proposed Operational Definition
,
2004
.
[14]
G. Jamieson.
Hypnosis and conscious states : the cognitive neuroscience perspective
,
2007
.
[15]
Harald Piron,et al.
Meditation Depth, Mental Health, and Personal Development
,
2003
.
[16]
S. Goenka,et al.
The art of living : Vipassana meditation as taught by S.N. Goenka
,
1990
.
[17]
J. C. Smith.
Meditation as psychotherapy: a new look at the evidence
,
1990
.
[18]
J. G. Hibben.
The Varieties of Religious Experience, A Study in Human Nature.
,
1903
.
[19]
D. Paulhus.
Two-component models of socially desirable responding.
,
1984
.
[20]
C. Germer,et al.
Mindfulness and psychotherapy
,
2005
.
[21]
Karl G. Jöreskog,et al.
Lisrel 8: User's Reference Guide
,
1997
.
[22]
R. Baer.
Mindfulness Training as a Clinical Intervention: A Conceptual and Empirical Review
,
2003
.
[23]
J. Kabat-Zinn,et al.
An outpatient program in behavioral medicine for chronic pain patients based on the practice of mindfulness meditation: theoretical considerations and preliminary results.
,
1982,
General hospital psychiatry.
[24]
H. Piron.
The Meditation Depth Index ( MEDI ) and the Meditation Depth Questionnaire ( MEDEQ ) by
,
2008
.
[25]
Douglas N. Jackson,et al.
Problems in Human Assessment
,
1978
.
[26]
Michael Grüninger,et al.
Introduction
,
2002,
CACM.
[27]
R. Davidson,et al.
Attentional and affective concomitants of meditation: a cross-sectional study.
,
1976,
Journal of abnormal psychology.
[28]
Derek Gifford-May,et al.
"Deep states" of meditation: Phenomenological reports of experience.
,
1994
.
[29]
Harald Walach,et al.
Measuring Mindfulness in Insight Meditation ( Vipassana ) and Meditation-Based Psychotherapy : The Development of the Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory ( FMI ) by
,
2008
.
[30]
Paul D. Wienpahl,et al.
The Three Pillars of Zen
,
1967
.
[31]
Karl G. Jöreskog,et al.
Lisrel 8: Structural Equation Modeling With the Simplis Command Language
,
1993
.
[32]
P. Bentler,et al.
Comparative fit indexes in structural models.
,
1990,
Psychological bulletin.
[33]
A. Tellegen,et al.
Openness to absorbing and self-altering experiences ("absorption"), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility.
,
1974,
Journal of abnormal psychology.
[34]
R. Walsh,et al.
An analysis of recent meditation research and suggestions for future directions
,
2003
.
[35]
Harald Walach,et al.
Measuring mindfulness in insight meditation (Vipassana) and meditationbased psychotherapy: The development of the Freiburg mindfulness inventory (FMI). Journal for Meditation and Meditation Research, .
,
2001
.
[36]
P. Grossman,et al.
Mindfulness-based stress reduction and health benefits. A meta-analysis.
,
2004,
Journal of psychosomatic research.
[37]
Chögyam Trungpa,et al.
Cutting Through Spiritual Materialism
,
1973
.
[38]
P. Bentler,et al.
Significance Tests and Goodness of Fit in the Analysis of Covariance Structures
,
1980
.
[39]
R. Mueller.
Basic Principles of Structural Equation Modeling: An Introduction to LISREL and EQS
,
1996
.