Learning without consciously knowing: Evidence from event-related potentials in sequence learning

This paper investigated how implicit and explicit knowledge is reflected in event-related potentials (ERPs) in sequence learning. ERPs were recorded during a serial reaction time task. The results showed that there were greater RT benefits for standard compared with deviant stimuli later than early on, indicating sequence learning. After training, more standard triplets were generated under inclusion than exclusion tests and more standard triplets under exclusion than chance level, indicating that participants acquired both explicit and implicit knowledge. However, deviant targets elicited enhanced N2 and P3 components for targets with explicit knowledge but a larger N2 effect for targets with implicit knowledge, revealing that implicit knowledge expresses itself in relatively early components (N2) and explicit knowledge in additional P3 components. The results help resolve current debate about the neural substrates supporting implicit and explicit learning.

[1]  R. Passingham,et al.  Relative blindsight in normal observers and the neural correlate of visual consciousness , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[2]  Steven Laureys,et al.  Cerebral correlates of explicit sequence learning. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[3]  Leonora Wilkinson,et al.  Intentional control and implicit sequence learning. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[4]  G. Remillard Pure perceptual-based sequence learning: a role for visuospatial attention. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[5]  Jonathan R. Folstein,et al.  Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: a review. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[6]  Axel Cleeremans,et al.  Implicit learning: news from the front , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[7]  Xiaolan Fu,et al.  Implicit sequence learning and conscious awareness , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.

[8]  M Eimer,et al.  Explicit and implicit learning of event sequences: evidence from event-related brain potentials. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[9]  K. Henke A model for memory systems based on processing modes rather than consciousness , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[10]  R. Barry,et al.  Movement-related potentials in the Go/NoGo task: The P3 reflects both cognitive and motor inhibition , 2008, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[11]  Cameron S. Carter,et al.  Conflict-related activity in the caudal anterior cingulate cortex in the absence of awareness , 2009, Biological Psychology.

[12]  I. Visser,et al.  The role of reversal frequency in learning noisy second order conditional sequences , 2010, Consciousness and Cognition.

[13]  Michael Snodgrass,et al.  Subliminal visual oddball stimuli evoke a P300 component , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[14]  Z. Dienes Subjective measures of unconscious knowledge. , 2008, Progress in brain research.

[15]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Functional Anatomy of Visuomotor Skill Learning in Human Subjects Examined with Positron Emission Tomography , 1996, The European journal of neuroscience.

[16]  Dianne C. Berry,et al.  Implicit Learning , 1993 .

[17]  Axel Cleeremans,et al.  Can sequence learning be implicit? New evidence with the process dissociation procedure , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[18]  Robert J. Barry,et al.  The auditory-evoked N2 and P3 components in the stop-signal task: Indices of inhibition, response-conflict or error-detection? , 2006, Brain and Cognition.

[19]  Z. Dienes,et al.  Executive control without conscious awareness: the cold control theory of hypnosis , 2007 .

[20]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Chunking processes in the learning of event sequences: Electrophysiological indicators , 2000, Memory & cognition.

[21]  J Hoffmann,et al.  Irrelevant response effects improve serial learning in serial reaction time tasks. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[22]  Xiaolan Fu,et al.  Can unconscious knowledge allow control in sequence learning? , 2010, Consciousness and Cognition.

[23]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  Direct comparison of neural systems mediating conscious and unconscious skill learning. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  Robert J. Barry,et al.  Response priming in the Go/NoGo task: The N2 reflects neither inhibition nor conflict , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[25]  Research report D in incidental and intentional learning of sensorimotor sequences as revealed by event-related brain potentials , 2003 .

[26]  Shelley Channon,et al.  Relationship between priming and recognition in deterministic and probabilistic sequence learning. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[27]  Axel Cleeremans,et al.  Striatum forever, despite sequence learning variability: A random effect analysis of PET data , 2000, Human brain mapping.

[28]  D. Rosenthal Consciousness and mind , 2005 .

[29]  Luis Jiménez Intention, attention, and consciousness in probabilistic sequence learning , 2003 .

[30]  Z. Dienes,et al.  Can musical transformations be implicitly learned , 2004 .

[31]  M. Jahanshahi,et al.  The striatum and probabilistic implicit sequence learning , 2007, Brain Research.

[32]  Minna Lehtonen,et al.  Independence of visual awareness from the scope of attention: an electrophysiological study. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[33]  Elisabeth Norman,et al.  Fringe consciousness in sequence learning: The influence of individual differences , 2006, Consciousness and Cognition.

[34]  Axel Mecklinger,et al.  Error and Deviance Processing in Implicit and Explicit Sequence Learning , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[35]  W. Fias,et al.  Disentangling perceptual from motor implicit sequence learning with a serial color-matching task , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[36]  Z. Dienes,et al.  Intentional control based on familiarity in artificial grammar learning , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.

[37]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Attention and stimulus characteristics determine the locus of motor-sequence encoding. A PET study. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[38]  Tomifusa Kuboki,et al.  Explicit knowledge and intention to learn in sequence learning: an event-related potential study , 2005, Neuroreport.

[39]  T. Goschke,et al.  Implicit learning of semantic category sequences: response-independent acquisition of abstract sequential regularities. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[40]  J. R. Vokey,et al.  Beyond dissociation logic: evidence for controlled and automatic influences in artificial grammar learning. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[41]  D. Rosenthal Consciousness and Higher‐Order Thought , 2006 .

[42]  Michael Ziessler,et al.  Response-effect learning as a major component of implicit serial learning , 1998 .

[43]  Axel Cleeremans,et al.  The neural correlates of implicit and explicit sequence learning: Interacting networks revealed by the process dissociation procedure. , 2005, Learning & memory.

[44]  D. Willingham,et al.  Implicit motor sequence learning is represented in response locations , 2000, Memory & cognition.

[45]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Brain Dynamics Underlying the Nonlinear Threshold for Access to Consciousness , 2007, PLoS biology.

[46]  M. Nissen,et al.  On the development of procedural knowledge. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[47]  J. L. Myers,et al.  Regression analyses of repeated measures data in cognitive research. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[48]  Peder J. Johnson,et al.  Assessing implicit learning with indirect tests: Determining what is learned about sequence structure. , 1994 .

[49]  Z. Diénès Understanding Psychology as a Science: An Introduction to Scientific and Statistical Inference , 2008 .

[50]  Peter Bullemer,et al.  On the development of procedural knowledge. , 1989 .

[51]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Learning the structure of event sequences. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[52]  Z. Dienes Bayesian Versus Orthodox Statistics: Which Side Are You On? , 2011, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[53]  C. Stern,et al.  An fMRI Study of the Role of the Medial Temporal Lobe in Implicit and Explicit Sequence Learning , 2003, Neuron.

[54]  Eric Soetens,et al.  Spatial processing and perceptual sequence learning in SRT tasks. , 2006, Experimental psychology.

[55]  M. Amorim,et al.  Conscious knowledge and changes in performance in sequence learning: evidence against dissociation. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[56]  G. Jamieson Hypnosis and conscious states : the cognitive neuroscience perspective , 2007 .

[57]  Luis Jiménez,et al.  Temporal effects in sequence learning , 2003 .

[58]  M. Nissen,et al.  Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.