Older adults' affective experiences across 100 days are less variable and less complex than younger adults'.

Older adults are often described as being more emotionally competent than younger adults, and higher levels of affect complexity are seen as an indicator of this competence. We argue, however, that once age differences in affect variability are taken into account, older adults' everyday affective experiences will be characterized by lower affect complexity when compared with younger adults'. In addition, reduced affect complexity seems more likely from a theoretical point of view. We tested this hypothesis with a study in which younger and older adults reported their momentary affect on 100 days. Affect complexity was examined using clusterwise simultaneous component analysis based on covariance matrices to take into account differences in affect variability. We found that in the majority of older adults (55%), structures of affect were comparatively simpler than those of younger adults because they were reduced to a positive affect component. Most remaining older adults (35%) were characterized by differentiated rather than undifferentiated affective responding, as were a considerable number of younger adults (43%). When affect variability was made comparable across age groups, affect complexity also became comparable across age groups. It is interesting that individuals with the least complex structures had the highest levels of well-being. We conclude that affective experiences are not only less variable in the majority of older adults, but also less complex. Implications for understanding emotions across the life span are discussed.

[1]  G. Labouvie-vief,et al.  Dynamic Integration , 2003 .

[2]  Eva Ceulemans,et al.  Clusterwise simultaneous component analysis for analyzing structural differences in multivariate multiblock data. , 2012, Psychological methods.

[3]  Christina Röcke,et al.  Intraindividual variability and stability of affect and well-being: Short-term and long-term change and stabilization processes. , 2013 .

[4]  S. Charles,et al.  Now you see it, now you don't: age differences in affective reactivity to social tensions. , 2009, Psychology and aging.

[5]  U. Lindenberger,et al.  Differences in the Between–Person and Within–Person Structures of Affect Are A Matter of Degree , 2015 .

[6]  J. Mikels,et al.  Poignancy: mixed emotional experience in the face of meaningful endings. , 2008, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[7]  Shu-Chen Li,et al.  Intraindividual variability in positive and negative affect over 45 days: do older adults fluctuate less than young adults? , 2009, Psychology and aging.

[8]  S. Charles,et al.  Better with age: Social relationships across adulthood , 2011, Journal of social and personal relationships.

[9]  J. Mikels,et al.  Meta-analysis of the age-related positivity effect: age differences in preferences for positive over negative information. , 2014, Psychology and aging.

[10]  Leibniz-Informationszentrum Wirtschaft Seeking Pleasure and Seeking Pain: Differences in Prohedonic and Contra-Hedonic Motivation From Adolescence to Old Age , 2009 .

[11]  M. Diehl,et al.  Emotion complexity and emotion regulation across adulthood , 2011, European journal of ageing.

[12]  J. Swendsen,et al.  Routinization and emotional well-being: an experience sampling investigation in an elderly French sample. , 2003, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[13]  M. Riediger,et al.  Do everyday affective experiences differ throughout adulthood? A review of ambulatory-assessment evidence , 2014 .

[14]  R. Ready,et al.  Emotional complexity in younger, midlife, and older adults. , 2008, Psychology and aging.

[15]  L. Carstensen,et al.  Emotional experience improves with age: evidence based on over 10 years of experience sampling. , 2011, Psychology and aging.

[16]  A. Ong,et al.  The complexity of emotions in later life. , 2004, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[17]  Heather L. Urry,et al.  Emotion Regulation in Older Age , 2010 .

[18]  L. Carstensen,et al.  Emotional experience in everyday life across the adult life span. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[19]  Alexandra M. Freund,et al.  The Handbook of Life-Span Development , 2010 .

[20]  S. Charles Viewing injustice: greater emotion heterogeneity with age. , 2005, Psychology and aging.

[21]  N. Consedine,et al.  Emotion experience and expression across the adult life span: insights from a multimodal assessment study. , 2006, Psychology and aging.

[22]  Stacey B. Scott,et al.  Age, stress, and emotional complexity: results from two studies of daily experiences. , 2014, Psychology and aging.

[23]  Eva Ceulemans,et al.  How to perform multiblock component analysis in practice , 2011, Behavior Research Methods.

[24]  U. Kunzmann,et al.  Age differences in emotional reactivity: the sample case of sadness. , 2005, Psychology and aging.

[25]  E. Ceulemans,et al.  Negative emotion differentiation: Its personality and well-being correlates and a comparison of different assessment methods , 2014, Cognition & emotion.

[26]  U. Lindenberger,et al.  Physical and emotional well-being and the balance of needed and received emotional support: age differences in a daily diary study. , 2013, Social science & medicine.

[27]  Auke Tellegen,et al.  The structure of mood change: An idiographic/nomothetic analysis. , 1982 .

[28]  Gary King,et al.  Matching as Nonparametric Preprocessing for Reducing Model Dependence in Parametric Causal Inference , 2007, Political Analysis.

[29]  G. Labouvie-vief,et al.  Dynamic Integration of Emotion and Cognition: Equilibrium Regulation in Development and Aging , 2010 .

[30]  Ulman Lindenberger,et al.  Normal aging dampens the link between intrusive thoughts and negative affect in reaction to daily stressors. , 2011, Psychology and aging.

[31]  U. Staudinger,et al.  Affective experience in adulthood and old age: The role of affective arousal and perceived affect regulation. , 2009, Psychology and aging.

[32]  P. Molenaar A Manifesto on Psychology as Idiographic Science: Bringing the Person Back Into Scientific Psychology, This Time Forever , 2004 .

[33]  M. Hautzinger Die CES-D Skala: Ein Depressionsme–sinstrument für Untersuchungen in der Allgemeinbevölkerung. , 1988 .

[34]  Henk A. L. Kiers,et al.  The Harris-Kaiser independent cluster rotation as a method for rotation to simple component weights , 1994 .

[35]  M. Rosenkoetter,et al.  Postretirement Use of Time , 2001 .

[36]  Ulman Lindenberger,et al.  On the relation of mean reaction time and intraindividual reaction time variability. , 2009, Psychology and aging.

[37]  L. F. Barrett,et al.  Knowing what you're feeling and knowing what to do about it: Mapping the relation between emotion differentiation and emotion regulation , 2001 .

[38]  C. Cooper,et al.  The structure of moods , 1992 .

[39]  Daniel Grühn,et al.  Time-based indicators of emotional complexity: interrelations and correlates. , 2013, Emotion.

[40]  Eva Ceulemans,et al.  A clusterwise simultaneous component method for capturing within-cluster differences in component variances and correlations. , 2013, The British journal of mathematical and statistical psychology.

[41]  A. Steptoe,et al.  Positive affect and distressed affect over the day in older people. , 2011, Psychology and aging.

[42]  F. Schmiedek,et al.  Life contexts make a difference: emotional stability in younger and older adults. , 2013, Psychology and aging.

[43]  D. Watson,et al.  Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.