Agency and Control for the Integration of a Virtual Tool into the Peripersonal Space
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Melvyn A Goodale,et al. Motor Force Field Learning Influences Visual Processing of Target Motion , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[2] M. Hallett,et al. The neural processes underlying self-agency. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.
[3] R. Ward,et al. Virtual limbs and body space: Critical features for the distinction between body space and near-body space. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[4] Silvia Bonifazi,et al. The role played by tool-use and tool-length on the Plastic Elongation of peri-hand space: a single case study , 2005, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[5] Nicholas P. Holmes,et al. Tool use changes multisensory interactions in seconds: evidence from the crossmodal congruency task , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.
[6] R. Held. Dissociation of visual functions by deprivation and rearrangement , 1968 .
[7] Denis Cousineau,et al. Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson's method , 2005 .
[8] W. T. Thach,et al. Throwing while looking through prisms. I. Focal olivocerebellar lesions impair adaptation. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[9] G. Rognini,et al. Extending the Body to Virtual Tools Using a Robotic Surgical Interface: Evidence from the Crossmodal Congruency Task , 2012, PloS one.
[10] C. Spence,et al. Tool-use changes multimodal spatial interactions between vision and touch in normal humans , 2002, Cognition.
[11] Tamar Flash,et al. Computational approaches to motor control , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[12] R. Klein,et al. The presence of a nonresponding effector increases inhibition of return , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[13] M. Masson,et al. Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[14] A. Maravita,et al. Tools for the body (schema) , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[15] V. Gallese,et al. Tool-use observation makes far objects ready-to-hand , 2011, Neuropsychologia.
[16] Andrea Serino,et al. Everyday use of the computer mouse extends peripersonal space representation , 2010, Neuropsychologia.
[17] M. Goodale,et al. Enhanced detection of visual targets on the hand and familiar tools , 2009, Neuropsychologia.
[18] Robert L. Sainburg,et al. Mechanisms underlying interlimb transfer of visuomotor rotations , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.
[19] R. Passingham,et al. That's My Hand! Activity in Premotor Cortex Reflects Feeling of Ownership of a Limb , 2004, Science.
[20] D H Brainard,et al. The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[21] Thomas A Carlson,et al. Rapid Assimilation of External Objects Into the Body Schema , 2010, Psychological science.
[22] M. Tanaka,et al. Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. , 1996, Neuroreport.
[23] Melvyn A. Goodale,et al. Vision in the palm of your hand , 2009, Neuropsychologia.
[24] Francesca Frassinetti,et al. Direct evidence from parietal extinction of enhancement of visual attention near a visible hand , 2000, Current Biology.
[25] Jon Driver,et al. Reaching with a tool extends visual–tactile interactions into far space: evidence from cross-modal extinction , 2001, Neuropsychologia.
[26] Catherine L. Reed,et al. Grab it! Biased attention in functional hand and tool space , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[27] R. Hari,et al. Preference of Personal to Extrapersonal Space in a Visuomotor Task , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[28] Andrea Serino,et al. Dynamic Size-Change of Peri-Hand Space Following Tool-Use: Determinants and Spatial Characteristics Revealed Through Cross-Modal Extinction , 2007, Cortex.
[29] Alessandro Farnè,et al. Peripersonal Space and Body Schema: Two Labels for the Same Concept? , 2009, Brain Topography.
[30] Nicholas P. Holmes,et al. Tool-Use: Capturing Multisensory Spatial Attention or Extending Multisensory Peripersonal Space? , 2007, Cortex.
[31] D G Pelli,et al. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[32] P. Haggard,et al. Visual processing and the bodily self. , 2008, Acta psychologica.
[33] A. Dufour,et al. Improved visual sensitivity in the perihand space , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.
[34] Kazuo Hiraki,et al. The parietal role in the sense of self-ownership with temporal discrepancy between visual and proprioceptive feedbacks , 2005, NeuroImage.
[35] P. Haggard,et al. Sense of Agency Primes Manual Motor Responses , 2009, Perception.
[36] Atsushi Iriki,et al. Shaping multisensory action–space with tools: evidence from patients with cross-modal extinction , 2005, Neuropsychologia.
[37] M. Kawato,et al. A hierarchical neural-network model for control and learning of voluntary movement , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.
[38] P. Haggard,et al. Facilitated Processing of Visual Stimuli Associated with the Body , 2004, Perception.
[39] Masao Ito. The Cerebellum And Neural Control , 1984 .
[40] M. Graziano. Where is my arm? The relative role of vision and proprioception in the neuronal representation of limb position. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[41] A. Berti,et al. When Far Becomes Near: Remapping of Space by Tool Use , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[42] Charles G. Gross,et al. Visual responses with and without fixation: neurons in premotor cortex encode spatial locations independently of eye position , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.