Visual control of stable and unstable loads: what is the feedback delay and extent of linear time‐invariant control?

Human balance is commonly described using linear‐time‐invariant (LTI) models. The feedback time delay determines the position of balance in the motor‐control hierarchy. The extent of LTI control illuminates the automaticity of the control process. Using non‐parametric analysis, we measured the feedback delay, extent of LTI control and visuo‐motor transfer function in six randomly disturbed, visuo‐manual compensatory tracking tasks analogous to standing with small mechanical perturbations and purely visual information. The delay depended primarily on load order (2nd: 220 ± 30 ms, 1st: 124 ± 20 ms), and secondarily on visual magnification (extent 2nd: 34 ms, 1st: 8 ms) and was unaffected by load stability. LTI control explained 1st order and stable loads relatively well. For unstable (85% passive stabilisation) 2nd order loads, LTI control accounted for 40% of manual output at 0.1 Hz decreasing below 10% as frequency increased through the important 1–3 Hz region where manual power and visuo‐motor gain are high. Visual control of unstable 2nd order loads incurs substantial feedback delays and the control process will not be LTI. These features do not result from exclusive use of visual inputs because we found much shorter delays and a greater degree of LTI control when subjects visually controlled a 1st order load. Rather, these results suggest that delay and variability are inevitable when more flexible, intentional mechanisms are required to control 2nd order unstable loads. The high variability of quiet standing, and movement generally, may be indicative of flexible, variable delay, intentional mechanisms rather than the automatic LTI responses usually reported in response to large perturbations.

[1]  S. Park,et al.  Feedback equilibrium control during human standing , 2005, Biological Cybernetics.

[2]  Ian David Loram,et al.  Human balancing of an inverted pendulum with a compliant linkage: neural control by anticipatory intermittent bias , 2003, The Journal of physiology.

[3]  Pietro G. Morasso,et al.  Internal models in the control of posture , 1999, Neural Networks.

[4]  John G Milton,et al.  On-off intermittency in a human balancing task. , 2002, Physical review letters.

[5]  R. Lee,et al.  Evidence for abnormal long-loop reflexes in rigid Parkinsonian patients , 1975, Brain Research.

[6]  R C Miall,et al.  The cerebellum, predictive control and motor coordination. , 2007, Novartis Foundation symposium.

[7]  P. Morasso,et al.  Direct measurement of ankle stiffness during quiet standing: implications for control modelling and clinical application. , 2005, Gait & posture.

[8]  T. Nagata,et al.  Characteristics of somatosensory feedback in postural control during standing , 2001, IEEE Transactions on Neural Systems and Rehabilitation Engineering.

[9]  Matthew C Tresch A balanced view of motor control , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[10]  D. Wolpert Probabilistic models in human sensorimotor control. , 2007, Human movement science.

[11]  P. D. Neilson,et al.  Internal models and intermittency: A theoretical account of human tracking behavior , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[12]  B. Day,et al.  Voluntary modification of automatic arm movements evoked by motion of a visual target , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  R. Fitzpatrick,et al.  Proprioceptive, visual and vestibular thresholds for the perception of sway during standing in humans. , 1994, The Journal of physiology.

[14]  J. Houk,et al.  Regulatory actions of human stretch reflex. , 1976, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  L. Stark,et al.  Sampling or intermittency in hand control system dynamics. , 1968, Biophysical journal.

[16]  P. Rack,et al.  Reflex responses at the human ankle: the importance of tendon compliance. , 1983, The Journal of physiology.

[17]  R. Fitzpatrick,et al.  Ankle stiffness of standing humans in response to imperceptible perturbation: reflex and task‐dependent components. , 1992, The Journal of physiology.

[18]  G. L. Pellecchia,et al.  Postural sway increases with attentional demands of concurrent cognitive task. , 2003, Gait & posture.

[19]  J. Kehne The Neural Basis of Motor Control , 1987, The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine.

[20]  N. Bleich,et al.  Short latency visual evoked potentials to flashes from light-emitting diodes. , 1995, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[21]  Constantinos N Maganaris,et al.  The passive, human calf muscles in relation to standing: the short range stiffness lies in the contractile component , 2007, The Journal of physiology.

[22]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Forward Models for Physiological Motor Control , 1996, Neural Networks.

[23]  Ian David Loram,et al.  Human balancing of an inverted pendulum: position control by small, ballistic‐like, throw and catch movements , 2002, The Journal of physiology.

[24]  J. Valls-Solé The circuitry of the human spinal cord: Its role in motor control and movement disorders Pierrot-Deseilligny E, Burke D, editors. Hardback. Cambridge University Press; 2005. 642 p. [ISBN: 13978052182581]. , 2008, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[25]  Ian David Loram,et al.  Paradoxical muscle movement during postural control. , 2009, Medicine and science in sports and exercise.

[26]  Ian David Loram,et al.  Reply from Ian D. Loram, Constantinos N. Maganaris and Martin Lakie , 2005 .

[27]  Martin Lakie,et al.  Manually controlled human balancing using visual, vestibular and proprioceptive senses involves a common, low frequency neural process , 2006, The Journal of physiology.

[28]  R. Miall,et al.  Adaptation to visual feedback delays in manual tracking: evidence against the Smith Predictor model of human visually guided action , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  Ian David Loram,et al.  Direct measurement of human ankle stiffness during quiet standing: the intrinsic mechanical stiffness is insufficient for stability , 2002, The Journal of physiology.

[30]  Peter J. Gawthrop Intermittent constrained predictive control of mechanical systems , 2004 .

[31]  Torrence D. J. Welch,et al.  A feedback model reproduces muscle activity during human postural responses to support-surface translations. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  F C T van der Helm,et al.  Observations from unperturbed closed loop systems cannot indicate causality. , 2005, The Journal of physiology.

[33]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Equilibrium-Point Control Hypothesis Examined by Measured Arm Stiffness During Multijoint Movement , 1996, Science.

[34]  Frank Moss,et al.  Medical technology: Balancing the unbalanced , 2003, Nature.

[35]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  An internal model for sensorimotor integration. , 1995, Science.

[36]  E. C. Poulton,et al.  Tracking skill and manual control , 1974 .

[37]  J. Rothwell Control of Human Voluntary Movement , 1994, Springer Netherlands.

[38]  Simon C Gandevia,et al.  Postural control at the human wrist , 2008, The Journal of physiology.

[39]  C. Marsden,et al.  Influence of voluntary intent on the human long-latency stretch reflex , 1980, Nature.

[40]  P. D. Neilson,et al.  Stochastic prediction in pursuit tracking: An experimental test of adaptive model theory , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[41]  Lennart Ljung,et al.  System Identification: Theory for the User , 1987 .

[42]  L. Nashner Adapting reflexes controlling the human posture , 1976, Experimental Brain Research.

[43]  D. McCloskey,et al.  Effects of prior instruction and anaesthesia on long‐latency responses to stretch in the long flexor of the human thumb. , 1985, The Journal of physiology.

[44]  A M Amjad,et al.  A framework for the analysis of mixed time series/point process data--theory and application to the study of physiological tremor, single motor unit discharges and electromyograms. , 1995, Progress in biophysics and molecular biology.

[45]  Taishin Nomura,et al.  Bounded stability of the quiet standing posture: an intermittent control model. , 2008, Human movement science.

[46]  R. Peterka Sensorimotor integration in human postural control. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[47]  Ian David Loram,et al.  Human balancing of an inverted pendulum: is sway size controlled by ankle impedance? , 2001, The Journal of physiology.

[48]  D. B. Lockhart,et al.  Optimal sensorimotor transformations for balance , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[49]  Constantinos N Maganaris,et al.  Active, non‐spring‐like muscle movements in human postural sway: how might paradoxical changes in muscle length be produced? , 2005, The Journal of physiology.

[50]  Constantinos N Maganaris,et al.  The passive, human calf muscles in relation to standing: the non‐linear decrease from short range to long range stiffness , 2007, The Journal of physiology.

[51]  S C Gandevia,et al.  Loop gain of reflexes controlling human standing measured with the use of postural and vestibular disturbances. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[52]  Peter J. Gawthrop,et al.  Intermittent model predictive control , 2007 .

[53]  Ian Postlethwaite,et al.  Multivariable Feedback Control: Analysis and Design , 1996 .

[54]  R E Kearney,et al.  The 'late' electromyographic response to limb displacement in man. I. Evidence for supraspinal contribution. , 1979, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[55]  Juan Luis Cabrera,et al.  Human stick balancing: tuning Lèvy flights to improve balance control. , 2004, Chaos.

[56]  P. Morasso,et al.  Can muscle stiffness alone stabilize upright standing? , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[57]  Daniel M Wolpert,et al.  Computational principles of sensorimotor control that minimize uncertainty and variability , 2007, The Journal of physiology.

[58]  R. Peterka,et al.  A new interpretation of spontaneous sway measures based on a simple model of human postural control. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[59]  Ian David Loram,et al.  Human postural sway results from frequent, ballistic bias impulses by soleus and gastrocnemius , 2005, The Journal of physiology.

[60]  Tim Kiemel,et al.  Slow dynamics of postural sway are in the feedback loop. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[61]  Peter J. Gawthrop,et al.  Intermittent Predictive Control of an Inverted Pendulum , 2006 .

[62]  Peter J. Gawthrop,et al.  Open-loop intermittent feedback control: practical continuous-time GPC , 1999 .

[63]  R. Fitzpatrick,et al.  Stable human standing with lower‐limb muscle afferents providing the only sensory input. , 1994, The Journal of physiology.

[64]  J. G. Hollands,et al.  Engineering Psychology and Human Performance , 1984 .

[65]  Yutaka Fukuoka,et al.  Role of Visual Feedback in Upright Posture Control , 2001, J. Robotics Mechatronics.

[66]  Peter J. Gawthrop,et al.  Predictive feedback control and Fitts–law , 2008, Biological Cybernetics.

[67]  B. Day,et al.  Stimulation of the human motor cortex through the scalp , 1991, Experimental physiology.

[68]  Ian David Loram,et al.  The frequency of human, manual adjustments in balancing an inverted pendulum is constrained by intrinsic physiological factors , 2006, The Journal of physiology.

[69]  Herman van der Kooij,et al.  Postural responses evoked by platform pertubations are dominated by continuous feedback. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[70]  P. Morasso,et al.  Body sway during quiet standing: is it the residual chattering of an intermittent stabilization process? , 2005, Human movement science.

[71]  R. Miall,et al.  Intermittency in human manual tracking tasks. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.

[72]  H. van der Kooij,et al.  Observations from unperturbed closed loop systems cannot indicate causality , 2005 .

[73]  Duane T. McRuer,et al.  A Review of Quasi-Linear Pilot Models , 1967 .