Recognition of moving and static faces by young infants.
暂无分享,去创建一个
Hervé Abdi | Yumiko Otsuka | So Kanazawa | Masami K Yamaguchi | Alice J O'Toole | Yukuo Konishi | H. Abdi | Y. Konishi | Y. Otsuka | S. Kanazawa | M. Yamaguchi | A. O’Toole
[1] C. Nelson,et al. Brain activity differentiates face and object processing in 6-month-old infants. , 1999, Developmental psychology.
[2] Nikolaus F. Troje,et al. Separation of texture and shape in images of faces for image coding and synthesis , 1997 .
[3] H. Bulf,et al. The role of kinetic information in newborns' perception of illusory contours. , 2007, Developmental science.
[4] Vicki Bruce,et al. Recognizing Famous Faces: Exploring the Benefits of Facial Motion , 2000 .
[5] A. E. Milewski,et al. Infants' discrimination of internal and external pattern elements. , 1976, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[6] F. L. Clayton,et al. Infant visual fixation on motion pictures of the human face. , 1968, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[7] A. J. Caron,et al. Infant discrimination of naturalistic emotional expressions: the role of face and voice. , 1988, Child development.
[8] A. Slater,et al. Visual perception and memory at birth , 1995 .
[9] A simplified oscillator suitable for auditory research and audiometry , 1943 .
[10] D. Lewkowicz,et al. Three‐month‐old infants learn arbitrary auditory–visual pairings between voices and faces , 2001 .
[11] J. Gibson. The Senses Considered As Perceptual Systems , 1967 .
[12] Scott P. Johnson,et al. Newborn Infants' Preference for Attractive Faces: The Role of Internal and External Facial Features. , 2000, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[13] G. Johansson. Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .
[14] H. Wallach,et al. The kinetic depth effect. , 1953, Journal of experimental psychology.
[15] E. Spelke,et al. Intermodal exploration and knowledge in infancy , 1979 .
[16] A. O'Toole,et al. Recognizing moving faces: a psychological and neural synthesis , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[17] M. Legerstee,et al. Five- and eight-month-old infants recognize their faces and voices as familiar and social stimuli. , 1998, Child development.
[18] Sabine Hunnius,et al. Gaze shifting in infancy: a longitudinal study using dynamic faces and abstract stimuli , 2004 .
[19] Vicki Bruce,et al. Evaluating the effectiveness of pixelation and blurring on masking the identity of familiar faces , 2001 .
[20] Infant attention to facial expressions and facial motion. , 1987, The Journal of genetic psychology.
[21] A. Walker-Andrews. Intermodal perception of expressive behaviors: Relation of eye and voice? , 1986 .
[22] L. Gogate,et al. Attention and memory for faces and actions in infancy: the salience of actions over faces in dynamic events. , 2002, Child development.
[23] Vicki Bruce,et al. The role of motion in learning new faces , 2003 .
[24] I. Bushnell. Modification of the externality effect in young infants. , 1979, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[25] P. Kellman. Perception of three-dimensional form by human infants , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.
[26] A. Walker,et al. Intermodal perception of expressive behaviors by human infants. , 1982, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[27] L. Bahrick,et al. Development of Visual Self-Recognition in Infancy , 1996 .
[28] C Owsley,et al. The Role of Motion in Infants' Perception of Solid Shape , 1983, Perception.
[29] V. Bruce,et al. The role of movement in the recognition of famous faces , 1999, Memory & cognition.
[30] A. Yonas,et al. Perception of three-dimensional shape specified by optic flow by 8-week-old infants , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.
[31] L. Bahrick,et al. Intermodal perception of adult and child faces and voices by infants. , 1998, Child development.
[32] A. Pick,et al. Intermodal perception of happy and angry expressive behaviors by seven-month-old infants. , 1992, Child development.
[33] J. Werker,et al. Infants' ability to match dynamic phonetic and gender information in the face and voice. , 2002, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[34] H. Bülthoff,et al. The use of facial motion and facial form during the processing of identity , 2003, Vision Research.
[35] E. Spelke,et al. Perception of partly occluded objects in infancy , 1983, Cognitive Psychology.
[36] A. Johnston,et al. Categorizing sex and identity from the biological motion of faces , 2001, Current Biology.
[37] Mark H. Johnson,et al. The effects of movement of internal features on infants' preferences for face-like stimuli , 1992 .
[38] L. Bahrick,et al. The development of infant learning about specific face-voice relations. , 2005, Developmental psychology.
[39] L. Bahrick,et al. Infants' Bimodal Perception of Gender , 1991 .
[40] A. Walker-Andrews. Infants' perception of expressive behaviors: differentiation of multimodal information. , 1997, Psychological bulletin.
[41] Alice J. O'Toole,et al. A video database of moving faces and people , 2005, IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence.
[42] F. Simion,et al. Newborns’ face recognition over changes in viewpoint , 2008, Cognition.
[43] E. Gibson,et al. An Ecological Approach to Perceptual Learning and Development , 2000 .
[44] A Yonas,et al. Infants’ sensitivity to kinetic information for three-dimensional object shape , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.
[45] O. Pascalis,et al. The Other-Race Effect Develops During Infancy , 2007, Psychological science.
[46] Z. Kourtzi,et al. A Matching Advantage for Dynamic Human Faces , 2002, Perception.
[47] Alan Johnston,et al. Infants' Discrimination of Faces by Using Biological Motion Cues , 2006, Perception.
[48] P D Eimas,et al. Perceptual cues that permit categorical differentiation of animal species by infants. , 1996, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[49] A. O'Toole,et al. Psychological and neural perspectives on the role of motion in face recognition. , 2003, Behavioral and cognitive neuroscience reviews.
[50] Yumiko Otsuka,et al. Neural activation to upright and inverted faces in infants measured by near infrared spectroscopy , 2007, NeuroImage.
[51] F. D. Horowitz,et al. The perception of facial expressions and stimulus motion by two- and five-month-old infants using holographic stimuli. , 1983, Child development.
[52] H. Bülthoff,et al. A search advantage for faces learned in motion , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.
[53] L. Bahrick,et al. Infant discrimination of faces in naturalistic events: actions are more salient than faces. , 2008, Developmental psychology.
[54] Irene Leo,et al. Newborns' face recognition: role of inner and outer facial features. , 2006, Child development.
[55] Philippe G. Schyns,et al. What goes up may come down: perceptual process and knowledge access in the organization of complex visual patterns by young infants , 2003, Cogn. Sci..
[56] Y. Konishi,et al. The effect of occlusion on motion integration in infants. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[57] Chiara Turati,et al. Evidence of the Face Inversion Effect in 4-Month-Old Infants. , 2004, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[58] Development of visual space perception: From perception to cognition , 1987 .
[59] V. Bruce,et al. The role of dynamic information in the recognition of unfamiliar faces , 1998, Memory & cognition.
[60] R. Kaufmann-Hayoz,et al. Infants' recognition of a face revealed through motion: contribution of internal facial movement and head movement. , 1987, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[61] Infants' perception of illusory contours in static and moving figures. , 2003, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[62] L. Bahrick,et al. Perceiving the Real World: Infants' Detection of and Memory for Social Information. , 2001, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[63] J. N. Bassili. Emotion recognition: the role of facial movement and the relative importance of upper and lower areas of the face. , 1979, Journal of personality and social psychology.