Spatiotemporal brain mapping of spatial attention effects on pattern‐reversal ERPs

Recordings of event‐related potentials (ERPs) were combined with structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to investigate the timing and localization of stimulus selection processes during visual‐spatial attention to pattern‐reversing gratings. Pattern reversals were presented in random order to the left and right visual fields at a rapid rate, while subjects attended to the reversals in one field at a time. On separate runs, stimuli were presented in the upper and lower visual quadrants. The earliest ERP component (C1, peaking at around 80 ms), which inverted in polarity for upper versus lower field stimuli and was localized in or near visual area V1, was not modulated by attention. In the latency range 80–250 ms, multiple components were elicited that were increased in amplitude by attention and were colocalized with fMRI activations in specific visual cortical areas. The principal anatomical sources of these attention‐sensitive components were localized by fMRI‐seeded dipole modeling as follows: P1 (ca. 100 ms—source in motion‐sensitive area MT+), C2 (ca. 130 ms—same source as C1), N1a (ca. 145 ms—source in horizontal intraparietal sulcus), N1b (ca. 165 ms—source in fusiform gyrus, area V4/V8), N1c (ca. 180 ms—source in posterior intraparietal sulcus, area V3A), and P2 (ca. 220 ms—multiple sources, including parieto‐occipital sulcus, area V6). These results support the hypothesis that spatial attention acts to amplify both feed‐forward and feedback signals in multiple visual areas of both the dorsal and ventral streams of processing. Hum Brain Mapp, 2011. © 2011 Wiley‐Liss, Inc.

[1]  C C Wood,et al.  Retinotopic organization of human visual cortex: departures from the classical model. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[2]  Justin M. Ales,et al.  V1 is not uniquely identified by polarity reversals of responses to upper and lower visual field stimuli , 2010, NeuroImage.

[3]  S J Luck,et al.  Spatial filtering during visual search: evidence from human electrophysiology. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[4]  Raja Parasuraman,et al.  Brain mechanisms of involuntary visuospatial attention: An event‐related potential study , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[5]  D. Regan Human brain electrophysiology: Evoked potentials and evoked magnetic fields in science and medicine , 1989 .

[6]  S Zeki,et al.  The clinical and functional measurement of cortical (in)activity in the visual brain, with special reference to the two subdivisions (V4 and V4 alpha) of the human colour centre. , 1999, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[7]  D. Spinelli,et al.  Spatiotemporal analysis of the cortical sources of the steady‐state visual evoked potential , 2007, Human brain mapping.

[8]  D. J. Felleman,et al.  Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. , 1991, Cerebral cortex.

[9]  Matthias M. Müller,et al.  The time course of cortical facilitation during cued shifts of spatial attention , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[10]  Donatella Spinelli,et al.  Impaired visual processing of contralesional stimuli in neglect patients: a visual-evoked potential study. , 2008, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  Steven A. Hillyard,et al.  Steady-State VEP and Attentional Visual Processing , 2003 .

[12]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Identification of early visual evoked potential generators by retinotopic and topographic analyses , 1994 .

[13]  Anders M. Dale,et al.  Cortical Surface-Based Analysis I. Segmentation and Surface Reconstruction , 1999, NeuroImage.

[14]  Johannes Jacobus Fahrenfort,et al.  The spatiotemporal profile of cortical processing leading up to visual perception. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[15]  A. Dale,et al.  The Retinotopy of Visual Spatial Attention , 1998, Neuron.

[16]  G. Mangun,et al.  The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[17]  A. Ioannides,et al.  Attention Modulates Earliest Responses in the Primary Auditory and Visual Cortices , 2008, Neuron.

[18]  Sabrina Pitzalis,et al.  The cortical visual area V6 in macaque and human brains , 2009, Journal of Physiology-Paris.

[19]  Steven A. Hillyard,et al.  Identification of the neural sources of the pattern-reversal VEP , 2005, NeuroImage.

[20]  Alice Mado Proverbio,et al.  The cognitive electrophysiology of mind and brain , 2003 .

[21]  S. Zeki,et al.  The position and topography of the human colour centre as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[22]  Matthew S. Tata,et al.  Attention modulates responses to motion reversals in human visual cortex , 2007, Neuroreport.

[23]  Hiroshi Shibasaki,et al.  Temporal profile of visual evoked responses to pattern-reversal stimulation analyzed with a whole-head magnetometer , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  S. A. Hillyard,et al.  Sustained division of the attentional spotlight , 2003, Nature.

[25]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Spatio-temporal analysis of feature-based attention. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[26]  A. Dale,et al.  Cortical Surface-Based Analysis II: Inflation, Flattening, and a Surface-Based Coordinate System , 1999, NeuroImage.

[27]  M. Posner,et al.  Attentional networks , 1994, Trends in Neurosciences.

[28]  R. Turner,et al.  Characterizing Evoked Hemodynamics with fMRI , 1995, NeuroImage.

[29]  Chantal Delon-Martin,et al.  Sequence of pattern onset responses in the human visual areas: an fMRI constrained VEP source analysis , 2004, NeuroImage.

[30]  Ken Barrett Evoked Potentials in Clinical Testing (2nd edition). By A. M. Halliday Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone. 1993. 752 pp. £70.00 (hb). , 1994 .

[31]  J W Belliveau,et al.  Borders of multiple visual areas in humans revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 1995, Science.

[32]  R. Tootell,et al.  Anatomical evidence for MT and additional cortical visual areas in humans. , 1995, Cerebral cortex.

[33]  D. Spinelli,et al.  Effects of sustained, voluntary attention on amplitude and latency of steady-state visual evoked potential: a costs and benefits analysis , 2002, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[34]  A. Ioannides,et al.  Early (N70m) Neuromagnetic Signal Topography and Striate and Extrastriate Generators Following Pattern Onset Quadrant Stimulation , 2001, NeuroImage.

[35]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Preface: Cerebral Cortex Has Come of Age , 1991 .

[36]  K. Fujii,et al.  Visualization for the analysis of fluid motion , 2005, J. Vis..

[37]  H. Spekreijse,et al.  Contextual modulation in primary visual cortex and scene perception , 2000 .

[38]  S J Luck,et al.  Effects of spatial cuing on luminance detectability: psychophysical and electrophysiological evidence for early selection. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[39]  Steven A. Hillyard,et al.  Effects of spatial cuing on luminance detectability: Psychophysical and electrophysiological evidence for early selection. , 1994 .

[40]  S. Thorpe,et al.  The Time Course of Visual Processing: From Early Perception to Decision-Making , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[41]  Richard B Buxton,et al.  Putting spatial attention on the map: timing and localization of stimulus selection processes in striate and extrastriate visual areas , 2001, Vision Research.

[42]  J. Maunsell,et al.  Effects of Attention on the Processing of Motion in Macaque Middle Temporal and Medial Superior Temporal Visual Cortical Areas , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  Raja Parasuraman,et al.  Dissociation of visual C1 and P1 components as a function of attentional load: An event-related potential study , 2010, Biological Psychology.

[44]  R Kakigi,et al.  Visual evoked cortical magnetic fields to pattern reversal stimulation. , 1997, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[45]  Shozo Tobimatsu,et al.  Visual evoked cortical magnetic responses to checkerboard pattern reversal stimulation: A study on the neural generators of N75, P100 and N145 , 1998, Journal of the Neurological Sciences.

[46]  J L Lancaster,et al.  The temporal dynamics of the effects in occipital cortex of visual-spatial selective attention. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[47]  Joseph B. Hopfinger,et al.  Interactions between endogenous and exogenous attention on cortical visual processing , 2006, NeuroImage.

[48]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Spatial Selective Attention Affects Early Extrastriate But Not Striate Components of the Visual Evoked Potential , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[49]  M. Sereno,et al.  Mapping of Contralateral Space in Retinotopic Coordinates by a Parietal Cortical Area in Humans , 2001, Science.

[50]  Jose M. Sanchez-Bornot,et al.  Model driven EEG/fMRI fusion of brain oscillations , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[51]  Yan Wang,et al.  Perceptual load interacts with involuntary attention at early processing stages: Event-related potential studies , 2009, NeuroImage.

[52]  Donatella Spinelli,et al.  Electrophysiological evidence for an early attentional mechanism in visual processing in humans , 1999, Vision Research.

[53]  Antigona Martínez,et al.  Source analysis of event-related cortical activity during visuo-spatial attention. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[54]  E. Halgren,et al.  Spatiotemporal mapping of brain activity by integration of multiple imaging modalities , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[55]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Cortical sources of the early components of the visual evoked potential , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[56]  E. Bizzi,et al.  The Cognitive Neurosciences , 1996 .

[57]  Colin Blakemore,et al.  Contrast dependence of motion-onset and pattern-reversal evoked potentials , 1995, Vision Research.

[58]  C. Galletti,et al.  Wide-Field Retinotopy Defines Human Cortical Visual Area V6 , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[59]  E. Vogel,et al.  Sensory gain control (amplification) as a mechanism of selective attention: electrophysiological and neuroimaging evidence. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[60]  D. Spinelli,et al.  Spatial attention has different effects on the magno- and parvocellular pathways. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[61]  C. Galletti,et al.  Human V6: The Medial Motion Area , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[62]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Delayed Striate Cortical Activation during Spatial Attention , 2002, Neuron.

[63]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Spatial attention modulates initial afferent activity in human primary visual cortex. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[64]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Retinotopy and color sensitivity in human visual cortical area V8 , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[65]  John J. Foxe,et al.  The Spatiotemporal Dynamics of Illusory Contour Processing: Combined High-Density Electrical Mapping, Source Analysis, and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[66]  M. Gazzaniga,et al.  The new cognitive neurosciences , 2000 .

[67]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Event-related brain potentials in the study of visual selective attention. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.