Gene–environment interaction on neural mechanisms of orthographic processing in Chinese children

The ability to process and identify visual words requires efficient orthographic processing of print, consisting of letters in alphabetic languages or characters in Chinese. The N170 is a robust neural marker for orthographic processes. Both genetic and environmental factors, such as home literacy, have been shown to influence orthographic processing at the behavioral level, but their relative contributions and interactions are not well understood. The present study aimed to reveal possible gene-by-environment interactions on orthographic processing at the behavioral and neural level in a normal children sample. Sixty 12 year old Chinese children from a 10-year longitudinal sample underwent an implicit visual-word color decision task on real words and stroke combinations. The ERP analysis focused on the increase of the occipito-temporal N170 to words compared to stroke combinations. The genetic analysis focused on two SNPs (rs1419228, rs1091047) in the gene DCDC2 based on previous findings linking these 2 SNPs to orthographic coding. Home literacy was measured previously as the number of children's books at home, when the children were at the age of 3. Relative to stroke combinations, real words evoked greater N170 in bilateral posterior brain regions. A significant interaction between rs1091047 and home literacy was observed on the changes of N170 comparing real words to stroke combinations in the left hemisphere. Particularly, children carrying the major allele "G" showed a similar N170 effect irrespective of their environment, while children carrying the minor allele "C" showed a smaller N170 effect in low home-literacy environment than those in good environment.

[1]  H. Shu,et al.  Morphological Awareness Uniquely Predicts Young Children's Chinese Character Recognition , 2003 .

[2]  P. Ahnert,et al.  The role of gene DCDC2 in German dyslexics , 2009, Annals of dyslexia.

[3]  R. Plomin,et al.  Genetic and environmental influences on maternal and sibling interaction in middle childhood: a sibling adoption study , 1992 .

[4]  Betty Ann Levy,et al.  Understanding print: early reading development and the contributions of home literacy experiences. , 2006, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[5]  Franck Ramus,et al.  Correction: The Influence of Socioeconomic Status on Children’s Brain Structure , 2012, PLoS ONE.

[6]  Bruno Rossion,et al.  Early lateralization and orientation tuning for face, word, and object processing in the visual cortex , 2003, NeuroImage.

[7]  D. Skuse,et al.  COMT Val108/158Met Modifies Mismatch Negativity and Cognitive Function in 22q11 Deletion Syndrome , 2005, Biological Psychiatry.

[8]  R. Salmelin,et al.  Dissociation of normal feature analysis and deficient processing of letter-strings in dyslexic adults. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[9]  Catherine McBride-Chang,et al.  Developmental trajectories of reading development and impairment from ages 3 to 8 years in Chinese children. , 2011, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[10]  Torkel Klingberg,et al.  Three Dyslexia Susceptibility Genes, DYX1C1, DCDC2, and KIAA0319, Affect Temporo-Parietal White Matter Structure , 2012, Biological Psychiatry.

[11]  Bruce D. McCandliss,et al.  The visual word form area: expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[12]  H. Shu,et al.  Chinese Children's Character Recognition: Visuo-Orthographic, Phonological Processing and Morphological Skills. , 2012 .

[13]  Franck Ramus,et al.  The Influence of Socioeconomic Status on Children’s Brain Structure , 2012, PloS one.

[14]  Bruce D. McCandliss,et al.  Fast, visual specialization for reading in English revealed by the topography of the N170 ERP response , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[15]  Heikki Lyytinen,et al.  A candidate gene for developmental dyslexia encodes a nuclear tetratricopeptide repeat domain protein dynamically regulated in brain , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[16]  S Lehéricy,et al.  The visual word form area: spatial and temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[17]  D. Fulker,et al.  Evidence for a genetic aetiology in reading disability of twins , 1987, Nature.

[18]  Peter Holmans,et al.  Strong evidence that KIAA0319 on chromosome 6p is a susceptibility gene for developmental dyslexia. , 2005, American journal of human genetics.

[19]  Margit Burmeister,et al.  Association of the DYX1C1 Dyslexia Susceptibility Gene with Orthography in the Chinese Population , 2012, PloS one.

[20]  Manuel A. R. Ferreira,et al.  PLINK: a tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. , 2007, American journal of human genetics.

[21]  Timothy C. Bates,et al.  Dyslexia and DYX1C1: deficits in reading and spelling associated with a missense mutation , 2010, Molecular Psychiatry.

[22]  R. Straub,et al.  Effect of COMT Val108/158 Met genotype on frontal lobe function and risk for schizophrenia , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[23]  R. Treiman,et al.  The beginnings of orthographic knowledge: Children's knowledge of double letters in words. , 1997 .

[24]  R. Plomin,et al.  From learning to read to reading to learn: substantial and stable genetic influence. , 2007, Child development.

[25]  Terje Sagvolden,et al.  Behavioral and Brain Functions. A new journal , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[26]  Yu Wang,et al.  Disruption of neuronal migration by RNAi of Dyx1c1 results in neocortical and hippocampal malformations. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[27]  Ellen M Wijsman,et al.  Evaluation of candidate genes for DYX1 and DYX2 in families with dyslexia , 2007, American journal of medical genetics. Part B, Neuropsychiatric genetics : the official publication of the International Society of Psychiatric Genetics.

[28]  Vince D. Calhoun,et al.  Polymorphism of DCDC2 Reveals Differences in Cortical Morphology of Healthy Individuals—A Preliminary Voxel Based Morphometry Study , 2008, Brain Imaging and Behavior.

[29]  Daniel Brandeis,et al.  Evidence for developmental changes in the visual word processing network beyond adolescence , 2006, NeuroImage.

[31]  B Müller-Myhsok,et al.  First genome-wide association scan on neurophysiological endophenotypes points to trans-regulation effects on SLC2A3 in dyslexic children , 2011, Molecular Psychiatry.

[32]  Matthew C Keller,et al.  A critical review of the first 10 years of candidate gene-by-environment interaction research in psychiatry. , 2011, The American journal of psychiatry.

[33]  H. Wimmer,et al.  A Common Left Occipito-Temporal Dysfunction in Developmental Dyslexia and Acquired Letter-By-Letter Reading? , 2010, PloS one.

[34]  C. Francks,et al.  Putative functional alleles of DYX1C1 are not associated with dyslexia susceptibility in a large sample of sibling pairs from the UK , 2004, Journal of Medical Genetics.

[35]  Nicholas G Martin,et al.  Dyslexia and DCDC2: normal variation in reading and spelling is associated with DCDC2 polymorphisms in an Australian population sample , 2010, European Journal of Human Genetics.

[36]  Richard C. Anderson,et al.  The Role of Home-Literacy Environment in Learning to Read Chinese , 2002 .

[37]  B. Pennington,et al.  Refinement of the 6p21.3 quantitative trait locus influencing dyslexia: linkage and association analyses , 2004, Human Genetics.

[38]  R. Olson,et al.  Genetic and Environmental Influences on Orthographic and Phonological Skills in Children With Reading Disabilities , 2001, Developmental neuropsychology.

[39]  Richard K Olson,et al.  Dyslexia: nature and nurture. , 2002, Dyslexia.

[40]  Daniel Brandeis,et al.  Impaired tuning of a fast occipito-temporal response for print in dyslexic children learning to read. , 2007, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[41]  M. Nobile,et al.  Effect of the catechol‐O‐methyltransferase val158met genotype on children’s early phases of facial stimuli processing , 2007, Genes, brain, and behavior.

[42]  R. Olson,et al.  Genetic and environmental influences on individual differences in printed word recognition. , 2003, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[43]  C. H. Chou,et al.  Association of the rs3743205 variant of DYX1C1 with dyslexia in Chinese children , 2011, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[44]  R. Olson,et al.  Genetic and Environmental Influences on Word Recognition and Spelling Deficits as a Function of Age , 2007, Behavior genetics.

[45]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The visual word form area: a prelexical representation of visual words in the fusiform gyrus , 2002, Neuroreport.

[46]  Bertram Müller-Myhsok,et al.  Association of a Rare Variant with Mismatch Negativity in a Region Between KIAA0319 and DCDC2 in Dyslexia , 2011, Behavior genetics.

[47]  Jean-Baptiste Poline,et al.  Genetic Variants of FOXP2 and KIAA0319/TTRAP/THEM2 Locus Are Associated with Altered Brain Activation in Distinct Language-Related Regions , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[48]  Catherine McBride-Chang,et al.  What Is in the Naming? A 5-Year Longitudinal Study of Early Rapid Naming and Phonological Sensitivity in Relation to Subsequent Reading Skills in Both Native Chinese and English as a Second Language , 2011 .

[49]  C. Ho,et al.  Cognitive profiling and preliminary subtyping in Chinese developmental dyslexia , 2004, Cognition.

[50]  E L Grigorenko,et al.  Developmental dyslexia: an update on genes, brains, and environments. , 2001, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[51]  Urs Maurer,et al.  Left-lateralized N170 Effects of Visual Expertise in Reading: Evidence from Japanese Syllabic and Logographic Scripts , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[52]  D. Schaid,et al.  Incidence of reading disability in a population-based birth cohort, 1976-1982, Rochester, Minn. , 2001, Mayo Clinic proceedings.

[53]  J. Pernier,et al.  ERP Manifestations of Processing Printed Words at Different Psycholinguistic Levels: Time Course and Scalp Distribution , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[54]  F. Ramus,et al.  From genes to behavior in developmental dyslexia , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[55]  Andreas Ziegler,et al.  Strong genetic evidence of DCDC2 as a susceptibility gene for dyslexia. , 2006, American journal of human genetics.

[56]  Evidence for developmental changes in categorization criteria for children and adults. , 1974, Human development.

[57]  C. Schatschneider,et al.  Reading Skills in Early Readers , 2006, Journal of learning disabilities.

[58]  B. Müller-Myhsok,et al.  Evidence for the Late MMN as a Neurophysiological Endophenotype for Dyslexia , 2012, PloS one.

[59]  Anne Bonnefond,et al.  Impaired visual expertise for print in French adults with dyslexia as shown by N170 tuning , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[60]  S. Fisher,et al.  Developmental dyslexia: genetic dissection of a complex cognitive trait , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[61]  Richard C. Anderson,et al.  Learning to See the Patterns in Chinese Characters , 2013 .

[62]  C. Francks,et al.  The genetic basis of dyslexia , 2002, The Lancet Neurology.

[63]  Tilo Kircher,et al.  The effect of the COMT val158met polymorphism on neural correlates of semantic verbal fluency , 2009, European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience.

[64]  S. Ceci,et al.  Nature-nurture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: a bioecological model. , 1994, Psychological review.

[65]  Jessica A. R. Logan,et al.  Genetic and environmental influences on the growth of early reading skills. , 2010, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[66]  Timothy C. Bates,et al.  Behaviour genetic analyses of reading and spelling: A component processes approach , 2004 .

[67]  Xu-Chu Weng,et al.  Selectivity of N170 in the left hemisphere as an electrophysiological marker for expertise in reading Chinese , 2012, Neuroscience Bulletin.

[68]  X. Weng,et al.  Left-lateralized N170 response to unpronounceable pseudo but not false Chinese characters—the key role of orthography , 2011, Neuroscience.

[69]  P. Skudlarski,et al.  DCDC2 is associated with reading disability and modulates neuronal development in the brain. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[70]  B. Pennington,et al.  Developmental dyslexia , 2012, The Lancet.

[71]  H. Shu,et al.  Phonological skills and vocabulary knowledge mediate socioeconomic status effects in predicting reading outcomes for Chinese children. , 2013, Developmental psychology.

[72]  Karl Magnus Petersson,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of impaired reading in dyslexic pre-adolescent children , 2012, Brain and Cognition.

[73]  Margaret J. Wright,et al.  A Haplotype Spanning KIAA0319 and TTRAP Is Associated with Normal Variation in Reading and Spelling Ability , 2007, Biological Psychiatry.

[74]  Richard Wade-Martins,et al.  The chromosome 6p22 haplotype associated with dyslexia reduces the expression of KIAA0319, a novel gene involved in neuronal migration. , 2006, Human molecular genetics.

[75]  Xuchu Weng,et al.  Left-lateralized early neurophysiological response for Chinese characters in young primary school children , 2011, Neuroscience Letters.