Self-Regulation and School Success

Some children fare better academically than others, even when family background and school and teacher quality are controlled for (Rivkin, Hanushek, & Kain, 2005 ). Variance in performance that persists when situational variables are held constant suggests that individual differences play an important role in determining whether children thrive or fail in school. In this chapter, we review research on individual differences in self-regulation and their relation to school success. Disciplines Psychology This book chapter is available at ScholarlyCommons: http://repository.upenn.edu/psychology_papers/3 208 Some children fare better academically than others, even when family background and school and teacher quality are controlled for (Rivkin, Hanushek, & Kain, 2005 ). Variance in performance that persists when situational variables are held constant suggests that individual dif erences play an important role in determining whether children thrive or fail in school. In this chapter, we review research on individual dif erences in selfregulation and their relation to school success. Historically, research on individual dif erences that bear on school success has focused on general intelligence. A century of empirical evidence has now unequivocally established that intelligence, dei ned as the “ability to understand complex ideas, to adapt ef ectively to the environment, to learn from experience, to engage in various forms of reasoning, to overcome obstacles by taking thought” (Neisser et al., 1996 , p. 77) has a monotonic, positive relationship with school success (Gottfredson, 2004; Kuncel, Ones, & Sackett, 2010 ; Lubinski, 2009 ). In contrast, the relation between school success and temperamental dif erences among children has only recently attracted serious attention from researchers. Temperament is typically dei ned as “constitutionally based individual dif erences in reactivity and self-regulation, in the domains of af ect, activity, and attention” (Rothbart & Bates, 2006 , p. 100). While assumed to have a substantial genetic basis, temperament is also inl uenced by experience and demonstrates both stability and change over time. h is chapter focuses on self-regulation because it is the dimension of temperament most reliably related to school success. We address several related questions: What is the relation between self-regulation and both educational attainment (e.g., years of education, high school completion) and Chapter 10 Self-Regulation and School Success Angela Lee Duckworth University of Pennsylvania Stephanie M. Carlson University of Minnesota Self-Regulation and School Success 209 achievement (e.g., teacher-assigned course grades, standardized achievement test scores)? Does self-regulation also predict job performance, health, and other dimensions of success in life? Finally, what progress has been made in deliberately cultivating self-regulatory competence in children? Naming, Defining, and Measuring Self-Regulation We dei ne self-regulation as the voluntary control of attentional, emotional, and behavioral impulses in the service of personally valued goals and standards. By specifying that goals and standards are personally valued, we do not mean that they are necessarily seli sh. On the contrary, self-regulation is required to adhere to goals and standards that are altruistic in nature (e.g., sharing a prize rather than keeping it all for oneself) as well as those that are not (e.g., receiving a larger treat for oneself rather than a smaller one). For clarity’s sake, we point out that we use the term “self-regulation” interchangeably with the terms self-control, self-discipline, and willpower – and suggest that the terms impulsiveness and impulsivity connote dei cits in self-regulatory competence. Of particular relevance to this chapter, we consider selfregulation to be coextensive with ef ortful control, a well-recognized aspect of temperament in children that has been dei ned as “the ability to inhibit a dominant response to perform a subdominant response, to detect errors, and to engage in planning” (Rothbart & Rueda, 2005 , p. 169). Crucially, in situations that tax self-regulation, at least two mutually exclusive responses are possible, and the weaker (i.e., subdominant) response is preferred to the stronger (i.e., dominant) impulse. While self-regulation is most certainly multi-dimensional in the sense of involving more than one distinct psychological process (Duckworth & Kern, 2011 ; Whiteside & Lynam, 2001 ; Zimmerman & Kitsantas, 2005 ), we suggest it is nevertheless a coherent higher-order construct (de Ridder, Lensvelt-Mulders, Finkenauer, Stok, & Baumeister, 2012; Heatherton & Wagner, 2011 ) and a proi table target of study alongside its component processes. In taxonomies of childhood temperament, self-regulation is typically distinguished from two factors that are more reactive and less voluntary in nature: negative emotionality (shyness, fear, sadness, etc.) and surgency (activity level, sensation seeking, positive emotion) (Rothbart & Rueda, 2005 ). h e location of self-regulation in omnibus taxonomies of adult personality is debatable (Revelle, 1997 ). At present, the most widely accepted organization for adult personality distinguishes i ve families of traits (the Big Five): Conscientiousness, Agreeableness, Extraversion, Emotional Stability, and Openness to Experience. Many psychologists consider self-regulation Duckworth and Carlson 210 to be identical – or nearly so – with Big Five Conscientiousness (Caspi & Shiner, 2006 ; Moi tt et al., 2011). Others have proposed that self-regulation relates to other Big Five factors as well. For instance, Whiteside and Lynam ( 2001 ) suggest that the tendency to think and plan before acting and the regulation of behavior in the face of frustration are both aspects of Big Five Conscientiousness, whereas the regulation of urgent, negative emotions corresponds to Big Five Emotional Stability, and the tendency to have strong impulses toward risky, exciting activities (which makes selfregulation more dii cult) relates to Big Five Extraversion. Additionally, in children, the regulation of impulses in the context of interactions with peers and adults has clear conceptual links to Big Five Agreeableness (Tsukayama, Duckworth, & Kim, 2011 ). Executive functioning overlaps conceptually with the temperament trait of ef ortful control, though the scientii c investigation of these two constructs tends to be segregated, with neuroscientists primarily interested in executive functioning and temperament researchers primarily concerned with ef ortful control (Rothbart & Rueda, 2005 ). Rueda, Posner, and Rothbart ( 2005 ) have argued that executive function (and in particular, the executive attention network, which monitors and resolves conl ict between other brain networks) and ef ortful control are concepts representing dif erent methodological approaches to studying self-regulation of behavior (see also Checa, Rodriguez-Bailon, & Rueda, 2008 ). Children who do better on direct tasks of executive function tend to be rated signii cantly higher in ef ortful control by their parents (Chang & Burns, 2005 ; Gerardi-Caulton, 2000 ; Gonzalez, Fuentes, Carranza, & Estevez, 2001 ; Rothbart, Ellis, Rueda, & Posner, 2003 ; Simonds, 2007 ). However, a recent meta-analysis suggests that in general, correlations between individual executive function tasks and questionnaire measures of self-control are small in size (e.g., r = .14 with informant-report ratings; Duckworth & Kern, 2011 ). Even when batteries of executive function tasks are used to improve reliability and validity (Carlson, Faja, & Beck, in press ), associates with informant ratings are only moderate in magnitude, suggesting that executive function is not the only contributing factor to self-controlled behavior. Historical Interest in Self-Regulation and School Success h e idea that self-regulation plays an important role in the classroom is not new. In a series of lectures addressed to Boston schoolteachers, William James ( 1899 ) stated that in “schoolroom work” there is inevitably “a large Self-Regulation and School Success 211 mass of material that must be dull and unexciting” (pp. 104–105). Further, “there is unquestionably a great native variety among individuals in the type of their attention. Some of us are naturally scatter-brained, and others follow easily a train of connected thoughts without temptation to swerve aside to other subjects” (p. 112). It follows, James argued, that a dispositional advantage in the capacity for sustained attention is tremendously benei cial in the classroom. Improbably, pioneers of intelligence testing were among the i rst to recognize the importance of self-regulation to academic performance. Binet and Simon ( 1916 ), architect of the i rst modern intelligence test, noted that performance in school: admits of other things than intelligence; to succeed in his studies, one must have qualities which depend on attention, will, and character; for example a certain docility, a regularity of habits, and especially continuity of ef ort . A child, even if intelligent, will learn little in class if he never listens, if he spends his time in playing tricks, in giggling, in playing truant. (p. 254, italics added) David Wechsler ( 1943 ), who several decades later helped usher intelligence testing into widespread clinical and educational practice, made similar observations about the unfortunate neglect of “non-intellective” factors which, in conjunction with general intelligence, determine intelligent behavior: When our scales measure the non-intellective as well as the intellectual factors in intelligence, they will more nearly measure what in actual life corresponds to intelligent behavior. Under these circumstances they might not be so ei cient in selecting individuals likely to succeed in Latin and geometry, but they should do a much better job in selecting those destined

[1]  A. Elliot,et al.  Handbook of Competence and Motivation , 2013 .

[2]  A. Lillard,et al.  Preschool children's development in classic Montessori, supplemented Montessori, and conventional programs. , 2012, Journal of school psychology.

[3]  Angela L Duckworth,et al.  What No Child Left Behind Leaves Behind: The Roles of IQ and Self-Control in Predicting Standardized Achievement Test Scores and Report Card Grades. , 2012, Journal of educational psychology.

[4]  Gerty J. L. M. Lensvelt-Mulders,et al.  Taking Stock of Self-Control , 2011, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[5]  A. Diamond,et al.  Interventions Shown to Aid Executive Function Development in Children 4 to 12 Years Old , 2011, Science.

[6]  Angela L. Duckworth,et al.  A Meta-Analysis of the Convergent Validity of Self-Control Measures. , 2011, Journal of research in personality.

[7]  T. Heatherton,et al.  Cognitive neuroscience of self-regulation failure , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[8]  S. M. Carlson Introduction to the special issue: executive function. , 2011, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[9]  J. Heckman,et al.  A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[10]  Angela L. Duckworth,et al.  Self‐regulation strategies improve self‐discipline in adolescents: benefits of mental contrasting and implementation intentions , 2011 .

[11]  J. Best,et al.  Effects of Physical Activity on Children's Executive Function: Contributions of Experimental Research on Aerobic Exercise. , 2010, Developmental review : DR.

[12]  Angela L. Duckworth,et al.  Establishing Causality Using Longitudinal Hierarchical Linear Modeling: An Illustration Predicting Achievement From Self-Control , 2010, Social psychological and personality science.

[13]  D. Ones,et al.  Individual differences as predictors of work, educational, and broad life outcomes , 2010 .

[14]  K. Lemery‐Chalfant,et al.  Prediction of Kindergartners' Academic Achievement from Their Effortful Control and Emotionality: Evidence for Direct and Moderated Relations. , 2010 .

[15]  H. Kuyper,et al.  Does homework behavior mediate the relation between personality and academic performance , 2010 .

[16]  Robert J. McMahon,et al.  The effects of a multiyear universal social-emotional learning program: The role of student and school characteristics. , 2010, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[17]  Alexandra Main,et al.  The Relations of Temperamental Effortful Control and Anger/Frustration to Chinese Children's Academic Achievement and Social Adjustment: A Longitudinal Study , 2010 .

[18]  Frederick J. Morrison,et al.  Early gender differences in self-regulation and academic achievement. , 2009 .

[19]  D. Lubinski Exceptional Cognitive Ability: The Phenotype , 2009, Behavior genetics.

[20]  Megan M. McClelland,et al.  A structured observation of behavioral self-regulation and its contribution to kindergarten outcomes. , 2009, Developmental psychology.

[21]  A. F. Kramer,et al.  The effect of acute treadmill walking on cognitive control and academic achievement in preadolescent children , 2009, Neuroscience.

[22]  A. Poropat A meta-analysis of the five-factor model of personality and academic performance. , 2009, Psychological bulletin.

[23]  Mark T. Greenberg,et al.  The Prosocial Classroom: Teacher Social and Emotional Competence in Relation to Student and Classroom Outcomes , 2009 .

[24]  A. Duckworth,et al.  (Over and) beyond high-stakes testing. , 2009, American Psychologist.

[25]  M. R. Rueda,et al.  Neurocognitive and Temperamental Systems of Self‐Regulation and Early Adolescents’ Social and Academic Outcomes , 2008 .

[26]  A. Kohn Why Self-Discipline Is Overrated: The (Troubling) Theory and Practice of Control from within. , 2008 .

[27]  N. Kuncel,et al.  Study Habits, Skills, and Attitudes: The Third Pillar Supporting Collegiate Academic Performance , 2008, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[28]  Thomas A. DiPrete,et al.  Gender Inequalities in Education , 2008 .

[29]  W. Steven Barnett,et al.  Educational effects of the Tools of the Mind curriculum: A randomized trial , 2008 .

[30]  Matthew J Borneman,et al.  High stakes testing in higher education and employment: appraising the evidence for validity and fairness. , 2008, The American psychologist.

[31]  Megan M. McClelland,et al.  Touch your toes! Developing a direct measure of behavioral regulation in early childhood , 2008 .

[32]  M. Reiser,et al.  Prediction of Children's Academic Competence From Their Effortful Control, Relationships, and Classroom Participation. , 2008, Journal of educational psychology.

[33]  A. Caspi,et al.  The Power of Personality: The Comparative Validity of Personality Traits, Socioeconomic Status, and Cognitive Ability for Predicting Important Life Outcomes , 2007, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[34]  A. Diamond,et al.  Preschool Program Improves Cognitive Control , 2007, Science.

[35]  G. Duncan,et al.  School readiness and later achievement. , 2007, Developmental psychology.

[36]  Megan M. McClelland,et al.  Links between behavioral regulation and preschoolers' literacy, vocabulary, and math skills. , 2007, Developmental psychology.

[37]  A. Gottfried,et al.  Temperamental Task Orientation: Relation to High School and College Educational Accomplishments. , 2007 .

[38]  J. Panksepp Can PLAY diminish ADHD and facilitate the construction of the social brain? , 2007, Journal of the Canadian Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry = Journal de l'Academie canadienne de psychiatrie de l'enfant et de l'adolescent.

[39]  C. Blair,et al.  Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. , 2007, Child development.

[40]  Sarah A. Hezlett,et al.  Standardized Tests Predict Graduate Students' Success , 2007, Science.

[41]  M. Mazzocco,et al.  A Longitudinal Assessment of Executive Function Skills and Their Association with Math Performance , 2007, Child neuropsychology : a journal on normal and abnormal development in childhood and adolescence.

[42]  Sarah E Hampson,et al.  A first large cohort study of personality trait stability over the 40 years between elementary school and midlife. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[43]  A. Lillard,et al.  Evaluating Montessori Education , 2006, Science.

[44]  Maureen A. Conard,et al.  Aptitude is not enough: How personality and behavior predict academic performance. , 2006 .

[45]  Angela L. Duckworth,et al.  Self-discipline gives girls the edge : Gender in self-discipline, grades, and achievement test scores , 2006 .

[46]  Angela L. Duckworth,et al.  Self-Discipline Outdoes IQ in Predicting Academic Performance of Adolescents , 2005, Psychological science.

[47]  F. Vitaro,et al.  Kindergarten Disruptive Behaviors, Protective Factors, and Educational Achievement by Early Adulthood. , 2005 .

[48]  Jerome L. Singer,et al.  Preschoolers' Imaginative Play as Precursor of Narrative Consciousness , 2005 .

[49]  M. Posner,et al.  The Development of Executive Attention: Contributions to the Emergence of Self-Regulation , 2005, Developmental neuropsychology.

[50]  B. Hamre,et al.  Can instructional and emotional support in the first-grade classroom make a difference for children at risk of school failure? , 2005, Child development.

[51]  Robert Perkins,et al.  The High School Transcript Study: A Decade of Change in Curricula and Achievement, 1990-2000. NCES 2004-455. , 2004 .

[52]  M. Posner,et al.  Developing mechanisms of temperamental effortful control. , 2003, Journal of personality.

[53]  J. Heckman,et al.  The Importance of Noncognitive Skills: Lessons from the GED Testing Program , 2001 .

[54]  Carmen González,et al.  Temperament and attention in the self-regulation of 7-year-old children , 2001 .

[55]  S. Whiteside,et al.  The Five Factor Model and impulsivity: using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity , 2001 .

[56]  Judith M. Pollack,et al.  Grades and Test Scores: Accounting for Observed Differences , 2000 .

[57]  G. Gerardi-Caulton,et al.  Sensitivity to spatial conflict and the development of self-regulation in children 24–36 months of age. , 2000 .

[58]  D. Fergusson,et al.  Early conduct problems and later life opportunities. , 1998, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[59]  E. Hanushek,et al.  Teachers, Schools, and Academic Achievement , 1998 .

[60]  William Damon Handbook of Child Psychology , 1997 .

[61]  H. Eysenck,et al.  The Scientific Study of Human Nature: Tribute to Hans J. Eysenck at Eighty , 1997 .

[62]  Elena Bodrova,et al.  Tools of the Mind: The Vygotskian Approach to Early Childhood Education , 1995 .

[63]  Kristin E. Voelkl,et al.  Disruptive and Inattentive-Withdrawn Behavior and Achievement among Fourth Graders , 1995, The Elementary School Journal.

[64]  Scott D. Johnson,et al.  Personality as a Predictor of College Performance , 1995 .

[65]  L. J. Stricker,et al.  Sex Differences in Predictions of College Grades from Scholastic Aptitude Test Scores. , 1993 .

[66]  D. Entwisle,et al.  First-grade classroom behavior: its short- and long-term consequences for school performance. , 1993, Child development.

[67]  M. Csíkszentmihályi,et al.  Motivation and academic achievement: the effects of personality traits and the quality of experience. , 1991, Journal of personality.

[68]  6. Cognitive and Emotional Growth through Play , 1990, The House of Make-Believe.

[69]  W. Mischel,et al.  Delay of gratification in children. , 1989, Science.

[70]  Warren W. Willingham,et al.  Success in College: The Role of Personal Qualities and Academic Ability , 1985 .

[71]  Barbara K. Keogh,et al.  Temperament , 1982 .

[72]  E. Saltz,et al.  Training disadvantaged preschoolers on various fantasy activities: effects on cognitive functioning and impulse control. , 1977, Child development.

[73]  W. Mischel,et al.  Substantive and structural elements of effective plans for self-control. , 1976 .

[74]  W. Mischel,et al.  Effects of temptation-inhibiting and task-facilitating plans on self-control. , 1976 .

[75]  W. Mischel,et al.  Plans to Resist Distraction. , 1975 .

[76]  F. J. Kelly,et al.  DELINQUENCY AND SCHOOL DROPOUT BEHAVIOR AS A FUNCTION OF IMPULSIVITY AND NONDOMINANT VALUES. , 1964, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[77]  Jelliffe.,et al.  THE DEVELOPMENT OF INTELLIGENCE IN CHILDREN (THE BINET-SIMON SCALE) , 1917 .

[78]  A. Binet,et al.  The Development of Intelligence in Children (the Binet-Simon Scale), and the Intelligence of the Feebleminded , 1916, The Psychological Clinic.

[79]  Charles Degarmo Talks to Teachers on Psychology, and to Students on some of Life's Ideals , 1899 .

[80]  J. Fantuzzo,et al.  CSRP's Impact on low-income preschoolers' preacademic skills: self-regulation as a mediating mechanism. , 2011, Child development.

[81]  Allison B. Dymnicki,et al.  The impact of enhancing students' social and emotional learning: a meta-analysis of school-based universal interventions. , 2011, Child development.

[82]  Allison B. Dymnicki,et al.  The Impact of Enhancing Students’ Social and Emotional Learning: A Meta-Analysis of School-Based Universal Interventions , 2011 .

[83]  S. Calkins,et al.  Child development at the intersection of emotion and cognition , 2010 .

[84]  Megan M. McClelland,et al.  Self-regulation and academic achievement in the transition to school. , 2010 .

[85]  A. Kramer,et al.  Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[86]  R. Robins,et al.  PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES Personality Predictors of Academic Outcomes: Big Five Correlates of GPA and SAT Scores , 2007 .

[87]  Kathleen D. Vohs,et al.  Self-regulation and the executive function: The self as controlling agent. , 2007 .

[88]  M. Elias,et al.  Educating People to Be Emotionally Intelligent , 2007 .

[89]  J. Simonds THE ROLE OF REWARD SENSITIVITY AND RESPONSE EXECUTION IN CHILDHOOD EXTRAVERSION , 2006 .

[90]  L. E. Berk,et al.  Make-Believe Play: Wellspring for Development of Self-Regulation. , 2006 .

[91]  B. Burns,et al.  Attention in preschoolers: associations with effortful control and motivation. , 2005, Child development.

[92]  D. Fergusson Gender Differences in Educational Achievement in a New Zealand Birth Cohort , 2004 .

[93]  L. Gottfredson Schools and the g Factor , 2004 .

[94]  G. Davies,et al.  Knowns and Unknowns , 2003 .

[95]  Deborah J. Leong,et al.  Tools of the Mind: A Case Study of Implementing the Vygotskian Approach in American Early Childhood and Primary Classrooms. Innodata Monographs 7. , 2001 .

[96]  B. Roberts,et al.  The rank-order consistency of personality traits from childhood to old age: a quantitative review of longitudinal studies. , 2000, Psychological bulletin.

[97]  Robert C. Pianta,et al.  Teachers’ judgments of problems in the transition to kindergarten , 2000 .

[98]  William Revelle,et al.  Extraversion and impulsivity: The lost dimension? , 1997 .

[99]  R. Sternberg,et al.  Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns. , 1996 .

[100]  E. Higgins,et al.  Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles. , 1996 .

[101]  Jerome L. Singer,et al.  The House of Make-Believe: Children's Play and the Developing Imagination , 1992 .

[102]  Roy P. Martin Activity level, distractibility, and persistence: Critical characteristics in early schooling. , 1989 .

[103]  J. Bates,et al.  Temperament in childhood. , 1989 .

[104]  D. Wechsler Non-intellective factors in general intelligence. , 1943 .

[105]  of Nebraska - Lincoln of Nebraska - Lincoln Temperamental attention and activity, classroom emotional Temperamental attention and activity, classroom emotional support, and academic achievement in third grade support, and academic achievement in third grade , 2022 .