Communicating Air Quality Information: Experimental Evaluation of Alternative Formats

A long-running effort in environmental communication is daily publication of a report on local air pollution in many American newspapers based on the Pollutant Standards Index (PSI). A 1998 proposal by the U.S. Environmental Protection Agency (U.S. EPA) to change the PSI prompted a survey experiment with 1,100 adults in Philadelphia, evaluating the proposed change's ability to better inform the populace. The effects of exposure to the old and new versions of the PSI, as well as health cautions and information about groups sensitive to air pollution, were compared with evaluation criteria suggested by Weinstein and Sandman (1993). Sample respondents had strong baseline concerns about air pollution. Descriptors of air quality (e.g., "good; " "unhealthy") were difficult to discriminate, particularly in the New format. Concern rose as hypothetical air pollution levels rose, but the New format (as well as PSI versions without health cautions or sensitive-group information) evoked a sharp discontinuity in concern between below- and above-standard pollution levels. Both Old and New formats reduced concern relative to no provision of PSI information at all, but the New format reduced concern significantly more than the Old version. No PSI format did particularly well at increasing knowledge of air pollution or decreasing intentions to be active outdoors during high pollution, contrary to the agency's aim. Although U.S. EPA has since adopted the new proposal as a national "Air Quality Index" requirement, the experiment's results illuminate the strengths and limitations of the new PSI as a means of informing citizens and motivating them to protect themselves.

[1]  Paul Slovic,et al.  Perceived risk, trust, and democracy , 1993 .

[2]  D. McKenzie‐Mohr,et al.  Fostering sustainable behavior through community-based social marketing. , 2000, The American psychologist.

[3]  N. Weinstein Unrealistic optimism about future life events , 1980 .

[4]  V. Smith,et al.  Can public information programs affect risk perceptions , 1990 .

[5]  D. Stieb,et al.  Do smog advisories work? Results of an evaluation of the Canadian Smog Advisory Program. , 1996, Canadian journal of public health = Revue canadienne de sante publique.

[6]  L. Jensen,et al.  Modifications of health behaviour in response to air pollution notifications in Copenhagen. , 1991, Social science & medicine.

[7]  Julie A. Rursch,et al.  The risks of "Putting the numbers in context": a cautionary tale. , 1994, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[8]  Karen Bickerstaff,et al.  Clearing the smog? Public responses to air‐quality information , 1999 .

[9]  Branden B Johnson,et al.  Gender and Race in Beliefs about Outdoor Air Pollution , 2002, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[10]  P M Sandman,et al.  High risk or low: how location on a "risk ladder" affects perceived risk. , 1994, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[11]  Baruch Fischhoff,et al.  Evaluating Risk Communications: Completing and Correcting Mental Models of Hazardous Processes, Part II , 1994 .

[12]  Paul Slovic,et al.  What Should We Know About Making Risk Comparisons?1 , 1990 .

[13]  N A Connelly,et al.  Evaluating Risk Communication: Examining Target Audience Perceptions About Four Presentation Formats for Fish Consumption Health Advisory Information , 1998, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[14]  Pieter Jan M. Stallen,et al.  Public Concern About Industrial Hazards , 1988 .

[15]  N D Weinstein,et al.  Why it won't happen to me: perceptions of risk factors and susceptibility. , 1984, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[16]  D. Roter,et al.  "If it bleeds it leads"? Attributes of TV health news stories that drive viewer attention. , 2000, Public health reports.

[17]  Lester B. Lave,et al.  Ethical Considerations in Risk Communication Practice and Research1 , 1990 .

[18]  M. G. Morgan,et al.  What Do We Know About Making Risk Comparisons , 1990 .

[19]  June A. Flora,et al.  Achieving Improvements in Overall Health Orientation , 1999 .

[20]  S D Colome,et al.  Psychological reactions to air pollution. , 1988, Environmental research.

[21]  J. Shapiro,et al.  Do Patients Want to Talk to Their Physicians about Organ Donation? Attitudes and Knowledge about Organ Donation: A Study of Orange County, California Residents , 1998, Journal of Community Health.

[22]  Peter M. Sandman,et al.  Some criteria for evaluating risk messages , 1993 .

[23]  D B Buller,et al.  Skin cancer prevention by parents of young children: health information sources, skin cancer knowledge, and sun-protection practices. , 1995, Oncology nursing forum.

[24]  Baruch Fischhoff,et al.  What Number is “Fifty‐Fifty”?: Redistributing Excessive 50% Responses in Elicited Probabilities , 2002, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[25]  Katherine E. Rowan,et al.  Why Rules for Risk Communication Are Not Enough: A Problem‐Solving Approach to Risk Communication , 1994 .

[26]  F R Johnson,et al.  Radon risk information and voluntary protection: evidence from a natural experiment. , 1987, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[27]  Jeanne D Edman,et al.  Measurement of exercise orientation in normalsubjects: gender and age differences , 1999 .