Reduced negativity effect in older adults' memory for emotional pictures: the heterogeneity-homogeneity list paradigm.

Using the heterogeneity-homogeneity list paradigm, the authors investigated 48 young adults' (20-30 years) and 48 older adults' (65-75 years) recognition memory for emotional pictures. The authors obtained no evidence for a positivity bias in older adults' memory: Age differences were primarily driven by older adults' diminished ability to remember negative pictures. The authors further found a strong effect of list types: Pictures, particularly neutral ones, were better recognized in homogeneous (blocked) lists than in heterogeneous (mixed) ones. Results confirm those of a previous study by D. Grühn, J. Smith, and P. B. Baltes (2005) that used a different type of to-be-remembered material, that is, pictures instead of words.

[1]  K. Vohs,et al.  Case Western Reserve University , 1990 .

[2]  U. Staudinger,et al.  Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology , 2007 .

[3]  M. Mather,et al.  Aging and emotional memory: the forgettable nature of negative images for older adults. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[4]  Alexandra M Freund,et al.  Life-management strategies of selection, optimization, and compensation: measurement by self-report and construct validity. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[5]  D. Isaacowitz Motivated Gaze , 2006 .

[6]  D. G. MacKay,et al.  Does emotion help or hinder immediate memory? Arousal versus priority-binding mechanisms. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[7]  Richard M. Lerner,et al.  Theoretical models of human development , 2006 .

[8]  Dinkar Sharma,et al.  Reversing the emotional Stroop effect reveals that it is not what it seems: the role of fast and slow components. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[9]  Edward B. Royzman,et al.  Negativity Bias, Negativity Dominance, and Contagion , 2001 .

[10]  D. Wechsler Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. , 1955 .

[11]  Natalie C. Ebner,et al.  Developmental changes in personal goal orientation from young to late adulthood: from striving for gains to maintenance and prevention of losses. , 2006, Psychology and aging.

[12]  J. Gross,et al.  Emotional suppression: physiology, self-report, and expressive behavior. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[13]  Mara Mather,et al.  Goal-directed memory: the role of cognitive control in older adults' emotional memory. , 2005, Psychology and aging.

[14]  Walter J. Perrig,et al.  Control of human behavior, mental processes and consciousness : essays in honor of the 60th birthday of August Flammer , 2000 .

[15]  M. Kisley,et al.  The negativity bias is eliminated in older adults: age-related reduction in event-related brain potentials associated with evaluative categorization. , 2006, Psychology and aging.

[16]  H. Aarts,et al.  On Wildebeests and Humans , 2003, Psychological science.

[17]  D. Watson,et al.  Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[18]  J. Gross Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[19]  Mara Mather,et al.  Aging and Attentional Biases for Emotional Faces , 2003, Psychological science.

[20]  Tammy English,et al.  Amygdala Responses to Emotionally Valenced Stimuli in Older and Younger Adults , 2004, Psychological science.

[21]  J. Gross,et al.  PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES Emotion Regulation and Memory: The Cognitive Costs of Keeping One's Cool , 2004 .

[22]  D. Wechsler WAIS-R manual : Wechsler adult intelligence scale-revised , 1981 .

[23]  P. Baltes,et al.  No aging bias favoring memory for positive material: evidence from a heterogeneity-homogeneity list paradigm using emotionally toned words. , 2005, Psychology and aging.

[24]  Alexandra M Freund,et al.  Age-differential motivational consequences of optimization versus compensation focus in younger and older adults. , 2006, Psychology and aging.

[25]  Hugh R. Wilson,et al.  Selective preference in visual fixation away from negative images in old age? An eye-tracking study. , 2006, Psychology and aging.

[26]  Laura L. Carstensen,et al.  Social and emotional patterns in adulthood: support for socioemotional selectivity theory. , 1992, Psychology and aging.

[27]  D. Algom,et al.  A rational look at the emotional stroop phenomenon: a generic slowdown, not a stroop effect. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[28]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events: the mobilization-minimization hypothesis. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[29]  Paul B. Baltes,et al.  Successful aging: Perspectives from the behavioral sciences , 1990 .

[30]  O. John,et al.  Automatic vigilance: the attention-grabbing power of negative social information. , 1991, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[31]  L. Carstensen,et al.  Taking time seriously. A theory of socioemotional selectivity. , 1999, The American psychologist.

[32]  Patricia A Reuter-Lorenz,et al.  Divergent trajectories in the aging mind: changes in working memory for affective versus visual information with age. , 2005, Psychology and aging.

[33]  P. Baltes,et al.  Psychological perspectives on successful aging: The model of selective optimization with compensation. , 1990 .

[34]  Introduction to the Special Section on Emotion--Cognition Interactions and the Aging Mind. , 2005 .

[35]  Paul B. Baltes,et al.  The orchestration of selection, optimization and compensation: An action–theoretical conceptualization of a theory of developmental regulation. , 2000 .