Cross-Language Intrusion Errors in Aging Bilinguals Reveal the Link Between Executive Control and Language Selection

Bilinguals outperform monolinguals on measures of executive control, but it is not known how bilingualism introduces these advantages. To address this question, we investigated whether language-control failures increase with aging-related declines in executive control. Eighteen younger and 18 older Spanish-English bilinguals completed a verbal-fluency task, in which they produced words in 18 categories (9 in each language), and a flanker task. Performance on both tasks exhibited robust effects of aging, but cross-language and within-language errors on the verbal-fluency task differed in a number of ways. Within-language errors occurred relatively often and decreased with higher levels of education in both younger and older bilinguals. In contrast, cross-language intrusions (e.g., inadvertently saying an English word on a Spanish-language trial) were rarely produced, were not associated with education level, and were strongly associated with flanker-task errors in older but not younger bilinguals. These results imply that executive control plays a role in maintaining language selection, but they also suggest the presence of independent forces that prevent language-selection errors.

[1]  Viorica Marian,et al.  Bilingualism influences inhibitory control in auditory comprehension , 2011, Cognition.

[2]  M. Strubell Studying bilinguals , 2010 .

[3]  D. Salmon,et al.  Accessibility of the nondominant language in picture naming: A counterintuitive effect of dementia on bilingual language production , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[4]  V. Ferreira,et al.  What causes the bilingual disadvantage in verbal fluency?: The dual-task analogy , 2010 .

[5]  F. Craik,et al.  Bilingual Minds , 2009, Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society.

[6]  Albert Costa,et al.  On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t , 2009, Cognition.

[7]  V. Ferreira,et al.  Should I stay or should I switch? A cost-benefit analysis of voluntary language switching in young and aging bilinguals. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[8]  Cathy J. Price,et al.  Structural Correlates of Semantic and Phonemic Fluency Ability in First and Second Languages , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[9]  M. Seghier,et al.  Language control and lexical competition in bilinguals: an event-related FMRI study. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[10]  T. Sandoval,et al.  More use almost always a means a smaller frequency effect: Aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. , 2008, Journal of memory and language.

[11]  Albert Costa,et al.  Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task , 2008, Cognition.

[12]  E. Peña,et al.  Lost in translation: methodological considerations in cross-cultural research. , 2007, Child development.

[13]  F. Craik,et al.  Dual-Modality Monitoring in a Classification Task: The Effects of Bilingualism and Ageing , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[14]  R. Turner,et al.  Language Control in the Bilingual Brain , 2006, Science.

[15]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Neurolinguistics: Structural plasticity in the bilingual brain , 2004, Nature.

[16]  F. Craik,et al.  Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: evidence from the Simon task. , 2004, Psychology and aging.

[17]  Albert Costa,et al.  Lexical Access in Bilingual Speech Production: Evidence from Language Switching in Highly Proficient Bilinguals and L2 Learners. , 2004 .

[18]  Jeremy R. Reynolds,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Transient and Sustained Cognitive Control during Task Switching , 2003, Neuron.

[19]  Jin Fan,et al.  Cognitive and Brain Consequences of Conflict , 2003, NeuroImage.

[20]  T. Gollan,et al.  Semantic and letter fluency in Spanish-English bilinguals. , 2002, Neuropsychology.

[21]  Bruce D. McCandliss,et al.  Testing the Efficiency and Independence of Attentional Networks , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[22]  Nanda Poulisse,et al.  Slips of the Tongue: Speech Errors in First and Second Language Production , 1999 .

[23]  Albert Costa,et al.  Lexical Selection in Bilinguals: Do Words in the Bilingual's Two Lexicons Compete for Selection? , 1999 .

[24]  Arturo E. Hernandez,et al.  Aging and Language Switching in Bilinguals , 1999 .

[25]  Willem J. M. Levelt,et al.  A theory of lexical access in speech production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[26]  J. Jescheniak,et al.  Discrete serial versus cascaded processing in lexical access in speech production : Further evidence from the coactivation of near-synonyms , 1998 .

[27]  D. Green,et al.  Lemma selection without inhibition of languages in bilingual speakers , 1998, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.

[28]  R. Heaton,et al.  Neuropsychological comparisons of Spanish-speaking participants from the U.S.–Mexico border region versus Spain , 1998, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[29]  R. R. Peterson,et al.  Lexical selection and phonological encoding during language production: Evidence for cascaded processing. , 1998 .

[30]  Matthew Flatt,et al.  PsyScope: An interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers , 1993 .

[31]  P. Lachenbruch Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed.) , 1989 .

[32]  S. Folstein,et al.  “Mini-mental state”: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician , 1975 .

[33]  N. Raz Aging of the brain and its impact on cognitive performance: Integration of structural and functional findings. , 2000 .