Characterizing operant hyperactivity in the Spontaneously Hypertensive Rat

BackgroundOperant hyperactivity, the emission of reinforced responses at an inordinately high rate, has been reported in children with ADHD and in the Spontaneously Hypertensive Rat (SHR), the most widely studied animal model of ADHD. The SHR emits behavior at hyperactive levels, relative to a normoactive strain, only when such behavior is seldom reinforced. Because of its dependence on rate of reinforcement, operant hyperactivity appears to be driven primarily by incentive motivation, not motoric capacity. This claim was evaluated in the present study using a novel strategy, based on the organization of behavior in bouts of reinforced responses separated by pauses.MethodMale SHR, Wistar-Kyoto (WKY) and Wistar rats (WIS) were exposed each to a multiple variable-interval schedule of sucrose reinforcement (12, 24, 48, 96, and 192 s) between post-natal days (PND) 48 and 93. Responding in each schedule was examined in two epochs, PND 58-62 and 89-93. Parameters of response-reinforcement functions (Herrnstein's hyperbola) and bout-organized behavior were estimated in each epoch.ResultsSHR emitted higher response rates than WKY and WIS, but only when rate of reinforcement was low (fewer than 2 reinforcers per minute), and particularly in the second epoch. Estimates of Herrnstein's hyperbola parameters suggested the primacy of motivational over motoric factors driving the response-rate differential. Across epochs and schedules, a more detailed analysis of response bouts by SHR revealed that these were shorter than those by WKY, but more frequent than those by WKY and WIS. Differences in bout length subsided between epochs, but differences in bout-initiation rate were exacerbated. These results were interpreted in light of robust evidence linking changes in bout-organization parameters and experimental manipulations of motivation and response-reinforcement contingency.ConclusionsOperant hyperactivity in SHR was confirmed. Although incentive motivation appears to play an important role in operant hyperactivity and motoric capacity cannot be ruled out as a factor, response-bout patterns suggest that operant hyperactivity is primarily driven by steeper delay-of-reinforcement gradients. Convergence of this conclusion with theoretical accounts of ADHD and with free-operant performance in children with ADHD supports the use of SHR as an animal model of ADHD.

[1]  T. Lehtimäki,et al.  Behavioral and Brain Functions , 2008 .

[2]  Berend Olivier,et al.  Spontaneously hypertensive rats do not predict symptoms of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder , 2006, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[3]  Terje Sagvolden,et al.  Behavioral validation of the spontaneously hypertensive rat (SHR) as an animal model of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (AD/HD) , 2000, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[4]  An experimental analysis of steady-state response rate components on variable ratio and variable interval schedules of reinforcement. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[5]  M. Konarska,et al.  Maternal effects on the development of spontaneous hypertension. , 1988, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[6]  S. Ferguson,et al.  A longitudinal study of short- and long-term activity levels in male and female spontaneously hypertensive, Wistar-Kyoto, and Sprague-Dawley rats. , 2003, Behavioral neuroscience.

[7]  R. Shull,et al.  Response rate viewed as engagement bouts: effects of relative reinforcement and schedule type. , 2001, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[8]  V. Russell Neurobiology of animal models of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[9]  Ryan D Ward,et al.  Resistance to change of responding maintained by unsignaled delays to reinforcement: a response-bout analysis. , 2006, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[10]  Joseph Biederman,et al.  The age-dependent decline of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analysis of follow-up studies , 2005, Psychological Medicine.

[11]  M. Sidman,et al.  Satiation effects under fixed-ratio schedules of reinforcement. , 1954, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[12]  P. Killeen Reinforcement frequency and contingency as factors in fixed-ratio behavior. , 1969, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[13]  R. Shull,et al.  Bouts of responding from variable-interval reinforcement of lever pressing by rats. , 2003, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[14]  W. J. Griffiths,et al.  Free-Operant Acquisition with Delayed Reinforcement , 1992 .

[15]  P. Killeen,et al.  Behavioral variability, elimination of responses, and delay-of-reinforcement gradients in SHR and WKY rats , 2007, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[16]  B. Amini,et al.  DIFFERENTIAL LOCOMOTOR RESPONSES IN MALE RATS FROM THREE STRAINS TO ACUTE METHYLPHENIDATE , 2004, The International journal of neuroscience.

[17]  J. Hagan Molecular and Functional Models in Neuropsychiatry , 2011 .

[18]  P. Killeen Models of trace decay, eligibility for reinforcement, and delay of reinforcement gradients, from exponential to hyperboloid , 2011, Behavioural Processes.

[19]  R. Herrnstein On the law of effect. , 1970, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[20]  B K Martens,et al.  The effects of variable-interval reinforcement on academic engagement: a demonstration of matching theory. , 1992, Journal of applied behavior analysis.

[21]  L. Spear,et al.  Periadolescence: age-dependent behavior and psychopharmacological responsivity in rats. , 1983, Developmental psychobiology.

[22]  S. L. Foster,et al.  Impaired Behavior Regulation Under Conditions of Concurrent Variable Schedules of Reinforcement in Children With ADHD , 2010, Journal of attention disorders.

[23]  C. Bradshaw,et al.  Relationship between response rate and reinforcement frequency in variable-interval schedules: III. The effect of d-amphetamine. , 1981, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[24]  John Evenden,et al.  The Behavior of Spontaneously Hypertensive and Wistar Kyoto Rats Under a Paced Fixed Consecutive Number Schedule of Reinforcement , 1999, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[25]  Espen Borgå Johansen,et al.  The spontaneously hypertensive rat model of ADHD – The importance of selecting the appropriate reference strain , 2009, Neuropharmacology.

[26]  T. Sagvolden,et al.  Spontaneously hypertensive rats (SHR) as a putative animal model of childhood hyperkinesis: SHR behavior compared to four other rat strains , 1993, Physiology & Behavior.

[27]  J. Wickens,et al.  Behavioral and Brain Functions Origins of Altered Reinforcement Effects in Adhd , 2022 .

[28]  S. Knardahl,et al.  Behavior of hypertensive and hyperactive rat strains: Hyperactivity is not unitarily determined , 1992, Physiology & Behavior.

[29]  Joseph Biederman,et al.  What Is the Prevalence of Adult ADHD? Results of a Population Screen of 966 Adults , 2005, Journal of attention disorders.

[30]  M. C. Newland,et al.  High-rate operant behavior in two mouse strains: A response-bout analysis , 2009, Behavioural Processes.

[31]  M. Reilly,et al.  Impulsive choice in a rodent model of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder , 2008, Behavioural Brain Research.

[32]  Janet B W Williams,et al.  Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders , 2013 .

[33]  Jonathan Williams,et al.  Dynamic behavioural changes in the Spontaneously Hyperactive Rat 1. Control by place, timing, and reinforcement rate , 2009, Behavioural Brain Research.

[34]  T. Sagvolden,et al.  Infrequent, but not frequent, reinforcers produce more variable responding and deficient sustained attention in young children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). , 2006, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[35]  M. Reilly,et al.  Response acquisition with delayed reinforcement in a rodent model of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) , 2006, Behavioural Brain Research.

[36]  H S HOFFMAN,et al.  A progression for generating variable-interval schedules. , 1962, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[37]  E Fantino,et al.  Choice: Some quantitative relations. , 1983, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[38]  Mehdi Farshbaf,et al.  Rodent Models of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder , 2005, Biological Psychiatry.

[39]  H. Feldman,et al.  Academic and educational outcomes of children with ADHD. , 2007, Ambulatory pediatrics : the official journal of the Ambulatory Pediatric Association.

[40]  Brooks Applegate,et al.  Developmental change in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in boys: A four-year longitudinal study , 1995, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[41]  Joseph Biederman,et al.  The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: is it an American condition? , 2003, World psychiatry : official journal of the World Psychiatric Association.

[42]  B. Alsop Problems with spontaneously hypertensive rats (SHR) as a model of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (AD/HD) , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[43]  E. Fantino,et al.  Unification of models for choice between delayed reinforcers. , 1990, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[44]  W. Barbaresi,et al.  Prevalence, recognition, and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a national sample of US children. , 2007, Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine.

[45]  M. Myers,et al.  Attenuation of hyperactivity in the spontaneously hypertensive rat by amphetamine. , 1982, Behavioral and neural biology.

[46]  David R. Anderson,et al.  Model selection and multimodel inference : a practical information-theoretic approach , 2003 .

[47]  C. Gentsch,et al.  Open field and elevated plus-maze: A behavioural comparison between spontaneously hypertensive (SHR) and Wistar-Kyoto (WKY) rats and the effects of chlordiazepoxide , 1987, Behavioural Brain Research.

[48]  R. Malott The effects of prefeeding in plain and chained fixed ratio schedules of reinforcement , 1966 .

[49]  M. A. Metzger,et al.  Frequent reward eliminates differences in activity between hyperkinetic rats and controls. , 1993, Behavioral and neural biology.

[50]  R J HERRNSTEIN,et al.  Relative and absolute strength of response as a function of frequency of reinforcement. , 1961, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[51]  P. Shizgal,et al.  Operant tempo varies with reinforcement rate: implications for measurement of reward efficacy , 2001, Behavioural Processes.

[52]  J. Dallery,et al.  Herrnstein's hyperbolic matching equation and behavioral pharmacology: review and critique , 2004, Behavioural pharmacology.

[53]  Heidi Aase,et al.  Altered reinforcement mechanisms in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder , 1998, Behavioural Brain Research.

[54]  Martin P. Paulus,et al.  Three independent factors characterize spontaneous rat motor activity , 1993, Behavioural Brain Research.

[55]  Matthew T Bowers,et al.  Interresponse time structures in variable-ratio and variable-interval schedules. , 2008, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[56]  O. Puciłowski,et al.  Behavioral studies in spontaneously hypertensive rats. , 1983, Behavioral and neural biology.

[57]  J. Wickens,et al.  Sensitivity to delay of reinforcement in two animal models of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) , 2009, Behavioural Brain Research.

[58]  P. Killeen,et al.  Evidence for impulsivity in the Spontaneously Hypertensive Rat drawn from complementary response-withholding tasks , 2008, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[59]  Y. Hsieh,et al.  Age-series characteristics of locomotor activities in spontaneously hypertensive rats: A comparison with the Wistar–Kyoto strain , 2008, Physiology & Behavior.

[60]  H. Feldman,et al.  Academic and educational outcomes of children with ADHD. , 2007, Journal of pediatric psychology.

[61]  R. Shull,et al.  Bouts of responding: the relation between bout rate and the rate of variable-interval reinforcement. , 2004, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[62]  R. Shull Bouts, Changeovers, and Units of Operant Behavior , 2011 .

[63]  I. J. Myung,et al.  Tutorial on maximum likelihood estimation , 2003 .

[64]  W. Hodos,et al.  Detection of the velocity of movement of visual stimuli by pigeons? , 1976, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[65]  Floyd C. Mace,et al.  Schedules of reinforcement , 2011 .

[66]  A. Scheres,et al.  Identifying the neurobiology of altered reinforcement sensitivity in ADHD: A review and research agenda , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[67]  J. Rabe-Jabłońska,et al.  [Affective disorders in the fourth edition of the classification of mental disorders prepared by the American Psychiatric Association -- diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders]. , 1993, Psychiatria polska.

[68]  Federico Sanabria,et al.  The isolation of motivational, motoric, and schedule effects on operant performance: a modeling approach. , 2011, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[69]  D S Blough,et al.  Interresponse time as a function of continuous variables: a new method and some data. , 1963, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[70]  C. Bradshaw,et al.  Relationship between response rate and reinforcement frequency in variable-interval schedules: II. Effect of the volume of sucrose reinforcement. , 1978, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[71]  R. Barkley Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. , 1997, Psychological bulletin.

[72]  R. Kessler,et al.  The prevalence and correlates of adult ADHD in the United States: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. , 2006, The American journal of psychiatry.

[73]  R. Herrnstein,et al.  Toward a law of response strength. , 1976 .

[74]  J. E. Fischel,et al.  The impact of preschool inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity on social and academic development: a review. , 2005, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[75]  B. Horta,et al.  The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: a systematic review and metaregression analysis. , 2007, The American journal of psychiatry.

[76]  V. Russell,et al.  A dynamic developmental theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) predominantly hyperactive/impulsive and combined subtypes. , 2005, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[77]  T. Sagvolden,et al.  Slower extinction of responses maintained by intra-cranial self-stimulation (ICSS) in an animal model of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) , 2005, Behavioural Brain Research.

[78]  K. Lattal Delayed reinforcement of operant behavior. , 2010, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[79]  T. Robbins,et al.  Animal models of ADHD. , 2011, Current topics in behavioral neurosciences.

[80]  R. Shull The sensitivity of response rate to the rate of variable-interval reinforcement for pigeons and rats: a review. , 2005, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[81]  V. Douglas,et al.  Effects of reward on delayed reaction time task performance of hyperactive children , 1983, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[82]  L. Klinger,et al.  Academic Underachievement and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: The Negative Impact of Symptom Severity on School Performance. , 2002 .

[83]  T. Sagvolden,et al.  Sex differences in operant discrimination behaviour in an animal model of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder , 1998, Behavioural Brain Research.

[84]  S. Knardahl,et al.  Open-field behavior of spontaneously hypertensive rats. , 1979, Behavioral and neural biology.

[85]  T. Sagvolden,et al.  Effects of delayed reinforcers on the behavior of an animal model of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) , 2005, Behavioural Brain Research.