Systemic functional adaptedness and domain-general cognition: broadening the scope of evolutionary psychology

Evolutionary psychology tends to be associated with a massively modular cognitive architecture. On this framework of human cognition, an assembly of specialized information processors called modules developed under selection pressures encountered throughout the phylogenic history of hominids. The coordinated activity of domain-specific modules carries out all the processes of belief fixation, abstract reasoning, and other facets of central cognition. Against the massive modularity thesis, I defend an account of systemic functional adaptedness, according to which non-modular systems emerged because of adaptive problems imposed by the intrinsic physiology of the evolving human brain. The proposed reformulation of evolutionary theorizing draws from neural network models and Cummins’ (J Philos 72(20):741–765, 1975) account of systemic functions to identify selection pressures that gave rise to non-modular, domain-general mechanisms in cognitive architecture.

[1]  P. Currie,et al.  Bristles before down: A new perspective on the functional origin of feathers , 2015, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.

[2]  D. Buss,et al.  Evolutionary Psychology: A New Paradigm for Psychological Science , 1995 .

[3]  S. Hurley Making sense of animals , 2006 .

[4]  D. Sperber The modularity of thought and the epidemiology of representations. , 1994 .

[5]  G. Lauder,et al.  Function without purpose , 1994 .

[6]  Kenneth Knoblauch,et al.  Quantifying the watercolor effect: from stimulus properties to neural models , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[7]  Dan Sperber,et al.  In defense of massive modularity , 2001 .

[8]  L. Cosmides,et al.  Mapping the mind: Origins of domain specificity: The evolution of functional organization , 1994 .

[9]  Peter Carruthers,et al.  On central cognition , 2014 .

[10]  C. Blakemore,et al.  Analysis of connectivity in the cat cerebral cortex , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[11]  S. Michael,et al.  Toward Mapping the Evolved Functional Organization of Mind and Brain , 2006 .

[12]  L. Schauble,et al.  Beyond Modularity: A Developmental Perspective on Cognitive Science. , 1994 .

[13]  P. Godfrey‐Smith Complexity and the function of mind in nature , 1996 .

[14]  Hyunjoong Kim,et al.  Functional Analysis I , 2017 .

[15]  J. Fuster Prefrontal Cortex , 2018 .

[16]  Changsong Zhou,et al.  Graph analysis of cortical networks reveals complex anatomical communication substrate. , 2009, Chaos.

[17]  David Badre,et al.  Analogical reasoning and prefrontal cortex: evidence for separable retrieval and integration mechanisms. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[18]  D. Weiskopf The Architecture of Higher Thought , 2014 .

[19]  Eleanor Rosch,et al.  Principles of Categorization , 1978 .

[20]  J. Wilson Sex and Death: An Introduction to Philosophy of Biology , 2000 .

[21]  Olaf Sporns,et al.  Network attributes for segregation and integration in the human brain , 2013, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[22]  L. Cosmides,et al.  THEPODULAR NATURE OF HUMAN INTELLIGENCE , 1997 .

[23]  Marcel A de Reus,et al.  Rich Club Organization and Intermodule Communication in the Cat Connectome , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  O. Sporns,et al.  Rich Club Organization of Macaque Cerebral Cortex and Its Role in Network Communication , 2012, PloS one.

[25]  S. Pinker How the Mind Works , 1999, Philosophy after Darwin.

[26]  P. Goldman-Rakic Cellular basis of working memory , 1995, Neuron.

[27]  O. Sporns,et al.  An Anatomical Substrate for Integration among Functional Networks in Human Cortex , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  Martijn P. van den Heuvel,et al.  Rich Club Organization and Cognitive Performance in Healthy Older Participants , 2015, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[29]  O. Sporns,et al.  Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[30]  Dan Sperber,et al.  Metarepresentations in an evolutionary perspective , 2000 .

[31]  G. Lauder,et al.  The Uses ofCausal Role Function in EvolutionaryBiology , 1994 .

[32]  D. Weiskopf Integrative Modeling and the Role of Neural Constraints , 2016, Philosophy of Science.

[33]  J. Fodor,et al.  Concepts: a potboiler , 1994, Cognition.

[34]  D. Buss The handbook of evolutionary psychology. , 2015 .

[35]  L. Cosmides,et al.  The psychological foundations of culture. , 1992 .

[36]  Michael L. Anderson,et al.  Neural reuse in the evolution and development of the brain: evidence for developmental homology? , 2013, Developmental psychobiology.

[37]  Peter Carruthers,et al.  The case for massively modular models of mind , 2004 .

[38]  Pete C. Trimmer,et al.  The evolution of decision rules in complex environments , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[39]  A. Luria Higher Cortical Functions in Man , 1980, Springer US.

[40]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[41]  P. Hagoort On Broca, brain, and binding: a new framework , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[42]  B. Baars A cognitive theory of consciousness , 1988 .

[43]  Justin L. Vincent,et al.  Evidence for a frontoparietal control system revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  Bill Brewer,et al.  Perception and reason , 2001 .

[45]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Is the rostro-caudal axis of the frontal lobe hierarchical? , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[46]  Daniel C. Krawczyk The cognition and neuroscience of relational reasoning , 2012, Brain Research.

[47]  S. Mithen The Prehistory of the Mind , 1996 .

[48]  E. Koechlin,et al.  The Architecture of Cognitive Control in the Human Prefrontal Cortex , 2003, Science.

[49]  Karen Neander,et al.  Minds without Meanings: An Essay on the Content of Concepts , 2017 .

[50]  Paul Sheldon Davies,et al.  The Nature of Natural Norms: Why Selected Functions are Systemic Capacity Functions , 2000 .

[51]  J. Kurths,et al.  Exploring Brain Function from Anatomical Connectivity , 2011, Front. Neurosci..

[52]  Gert Cauwenberghs,et al.  Neuromorphic Silicon Neuron Circuits , 2011, Front. Neurosci.

[53]  O. Sporns,et al.  Rich-Club Organization of the Human Connectome , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[54]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. , 2012, Annual review of neuroscience.

[55]  L. Cosmides The logic of social exchange: Has natural selection shaped how humans reason? Studies with the Wason selection task , 1989, Cognition.

[56]  G. Harman,et al.  Change in View: Principles of Reasoning. , 1988 .

[57]  O. Sporns,et al.  The economy of brain network organization , 2012, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[58]  G. Rees The new phrenology: the limits of localizing cognitive processes in the brain , 2002 .

[59]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  Irresistible environment meets immovable neurons , 1997, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[60]  Jessica R. Cohen,et al.  The Segregation and Integration of Distinct Brain Networks and Their Relationship to Cognition , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[61]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Domain-Specific Knowledge Systems in the Brain: The Animate-Inanimate Distinction , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[62]  K. Holyoak,et al.  The Oxford handbook of thinking and reasoning , 2012 .

[63]  David J. Buller Adapting Minds: Evolutionary Psychology and the Persistent Quest for Human Nature , 2005 .

[64]  B. Baars The conscious access hypothesis: origins and recent evidence , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[65]  J. Fodor,et al.  What Darwin Got Wrong , 2010 .

[66]  J. Prinz Is the mind really modular , 2005 .

[67]  G. Gigerenzer,et al.  Domain-specific reasoning: Social contracts, cheating, and perspective change , 1992, Cognition.

[68]  J. Duncan The multiple-demand (MD) system of the primate brain: mental programs for intelligent behaviour , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[69]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[70]  Armin W. Schulz Structural Flaws: Massive Modularity and the Argument from Design , 2008, The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science.

[71]  J. Fodor The Modularity of mind. An essay on faculty psychology , 1986 .

[72]  Krzysztof J. Gorgolewski,et al.  The Dynamics of Functional Brain Networks: Integrated Network States during Cognitive Task Performance , 2015, Neuron.

[73]  Michael W. Deem,et al.  Brain Modularity Mediates the Relation between Task Complexity and Performance , 2017, bioRxiv.

[74]  L. Cosmides,et al.  9 Toward Mapping the Evolved Functional Organization of Mind and Brain , 2000 .

[75]  James H. Fetzer,et al.  Logical reasoning and domain specificity , 1995 .

[76]  S. Gould,et al.  Exaptation—a Missing Term in the Science of Form , 1982, Paleobiology.

[77]  M. Michel A role for the anterior insular cortex in the global neuronal workspace model of consciousness , 2017, Consciousness and Cognition.

[78]  P. Godfrey‐Smith,et al.  Philosophy of Biology , 2011 .

[79]  E. Tricomi,et al.  Are You Smarter Than a Teenager? Maybe Not When It Comes to Reinforcement Learning , 2016, Neuron.

[80]  N. Daw,et al.  Deciding How To Decide: Self-Control and Meta-Decision Making , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[81]  J. Elman,et al.  Rethinking Innateness: A Connectionist Perspective on Development , 1996 .

[82]  Marcus Kaiser,et al.  Nonoptimal Component Placement, but Short Processing Paths, due to Long-Distance Projections in Neural Systems , 2006, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[83]  L Cosmides,et al.  CHARACTERIZING H U M A N PSYCHOLOGICAL ADAPTATIONS , 2005 .

[84]  O. Sporns Discovering the Human Connectome , 2012 .

[85]  Daniel C. Dennett,et al.  Multi-use and constraints from original use , 2010, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[86]  D. Weiskopf Concepts and the Modularity of Thought , 2010 .

[87]  M. V. D. Heuvel,et al.  Simulated rich club lesioning in brain networks: a scaffold for communication and integration? , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[88]  Gustavo Deco,et al.  Rich club organization supports a diverse set of functional network configurations , 2014, NeuroImage.

[89]  Oliver Obst,et al.  The Müller-Lyer Illusion in a Computational Model of Biological Object Recognition , 2013, PloS one.

[90]  Daniel C. Krawczyk Reasoning: The Neuroscience of How We Think , 2017 .

[91]  Clifford Werier Consciousness and Cognition in Shakespeare and Beyond , 2016 .

[92]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[93]  K. Holyoak Analogy and Relational Reasoning , 2012 .

[94]  James Woodward,et al.  The Mind is Not (Just) a System of Modules Shaped (Just) by Natural Selection , 2004 .

[95]  Samuel D. Carpenter,et al.  Structural and Functional Rich Club Organization of the Brain in Children and Adults , 2014, PloS one.

[96]  Gustavo Deco,et al.  Cortical rich club regions can organize state-dependent functional network formation by engaging in oscillatory behavior , 2017, NeuroImage.

[97]  L. Uddin Salience processing and insular cortical function and dysfunction , 2014, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[98]  H. Barrett The Shape of Thought: How Mental Adaptations Evolve , 2015 .

[99]  Yihong Yang,et al.  The Rich-Club Organization in Rat Functional Brain Network to Balance Between Communication Cost and Efficiency , 2018, Cerebral cortex.

[100]  Pablo Rychter Modularidad y teoría computacional de la mente en la obra de Jerry Fodor: Nota crítica en torno a The Mind Doesn't Work that Way , 2002 .

[101]  M. Wheeler,et al.  Evolutionary psychology's grain problem and the cognitive neuroscience of reasoning. , 2003 .

[102]  Angela R Laird,et al.  Brainmap taxonomy of experimental design: Description and evaluation , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[103]  Jonathan D. Power,et al.  Multi-task connectivity reveals flexible hubs for adaptive task control , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.

[104]  G. McCulloch,et al.  The Varieties of Reference. , 1984 .

[105]  D. Nettle A Module for Metaphor? The Site of Imagination in the Architecture of the Mind , 2007 .

[106]  J. Changeux,et al.  Experimental and Theoretical Approaches to Conscious Processing , 2011, Neuron.

[107]  Manfred G Kitzbichler,et al.  Cognitive Effort Drives Workspace Configuration of Human Brain Functional Networks , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[108]  Dan Sperber,et al.  Modularity and relevance: How can a massively modular mind be flexible and context-sensitive? , 2004 .

[109]  R. Samuels Evolutionary Psychology and the Massive Modularity Hypothesis , 1998, The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science.

[110]  John Zerilli Against the “system” module , 2017 .

[111]  H Clark Barrett,et al.  A hierarchical model of the evolution of human brain specializations , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[112]  Bernhard A. Sabel,et al.  Dynamic reorganization of brain functional networks during cognition , 2015, NeuroImage.

[113]  Kim Sterelny,et al.  Sex and Death. An Introduction to Philosophy of Biology (M. Matthen) , 1999 .

[114]  Peter Carruthers,et al.  Evolution of working memory , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[115]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Swinging in the brain: shared neural substrates for behaviors related to sequencing and music , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[116]  O. Sporns,et al.  High-cost, high-capacity backbone for global brain communication , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[117]  Michael L. Anderson,et al.  Quantifying the diversity of neural activations in individual brain regions , 2011, CogSci.

[118]  N. Unsworth,et al.  A locus coeruleus-norepinephrine account of individual differences in working memory capacity and attention control , 2017, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[119]  L. Cosmides,et al.  THE EMERGENCE OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY: WHAT IS AT STAKE? , 2022 .

[120]  D. Chklovskii,et al.  Maps in the brain: what can we learn from them? , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[121]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Dynamic cooperation and competition between brain systems during cognitive control , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[122]  O. Sporns Networks of the Brain , 2010 .

[123]  Suzana Herculano-Houzel,et al.  The Human Advantage: A New Understanding of How Our Brain Became Remarkable , 2016 .

[124]  D. Symons On the use and misuse of Darwinism in the study of human behavior. , 1992 .

[125]  Evelina Fedorenko The role of domain-general cognitive control in language comprehension , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[126]  Elliott Sober The Nature of Selection: Evolutionary Theory in Philosophical Focus , 1986 .

[127]  J. Fodor,et al.  The red herring and the pet fish: why concepts still can't be prototypes , 1996, Cognition.

[128]  L. Cosmides,et al.  Mapping the mind: Origins of domain specificity: The evolution of functional organization , 1994 .

[129]  B. Baars In the theater of consciousness : the workspace of the mind , 1997 .

[130]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Domain-Specific Knowledge Systems in the Brain: The Animate-Inanimate Distinction , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.