What affects detectability of lesion–deficit relationships in lesion studies?

Elucidating the brain basis for psychological processes and behavior is a fundamental aim of cognitive neuroscience. The lesion method, using voxel-based statistical analysis, is an important approach to this goal, identifying neural structures that are necessary for the support of specific mental operations, and complementing the strengths of functional imaging techniques. Lesion coverage in a population is by nature spatially heterogeneous and biased, systematically affecting the ability of lesion–deficit correlation methods to detect and localize functional associations. We have developed a simulator that allows investigators to model parameters in a lesion–deficit study and characterize the statistical bias in lesion deficit detection coverage that will result from specific assumptions. We used the simulator to assess the signal detection properties and localization accuracy of standard lesion–deficit correlation methods, under a simple truth model — that a critical region of interest (CR), when damaged, gives rise to a deficit. We considered voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping (VLSM) and proportional MAP-3 (PM3). Using regression analysis, we examined if the pattern of outcome statistics can be explained by simulation parameters, factors that are inherent to anatomic parcels, and lesion coverage of the population, which consisted of a representative sample of 351 subjects drawn from the Iowa Patient Registry. We examined the effect of using nonparametric versus parametric statistics to obtain thresholded maps and the effect of correcting for multiple comparisons using false discovery rate or cluster-based correction. Our results, which are derived from samples of realistic lesions, indicate that even a simple truth model yields localization errors that are systematic and pervasive, averaging 2 cm in the standard anatomic space, and tending to be directed towards areas of greater anatomic coverage. This displacement positions the center of mass of the detected region in a different anatomical region 87% of the time. This basic result is not affected by the choice of PM3 vs VLSM as the fundamental approach, nor is localization error ameliorated by incorporation of lesion size as a covariate in the VLSM approach, or by data distribution-driven approaches to controlling multiple spatial comparisons (false discovery rate or cluster-based correction approaches). Our simulations offer a quantitative basis for interpreting lesion studies in cognitive neuroscience. We suggest ways in which lesion simulation and analysis frameworks could be productively extended.

[1]  A R Damasio,et al.  A Neural Basis for the Retrieval of Words for Actions , 2001, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[2]  Hanna Damasio,et al.  Neural systems for recognition of emotional prosody: a 3-D lesion study. , 2002, Emotion.

[3]  Anders M. Dale,et al.  An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest , 2006, NeuroImage.

[4]  R. Adolphs,et al.  NEURAL CORRELATES OF CONCEPTUAL KNOWLEDGE FOR ACTIONS , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[5]  R. Adolphs,et al.  Neural systems behind word and concept retrieval , 2004, Cognition.

[6]  G. Rees,et al.  Human brain lesion-deficit inference remapped , 2014, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[7]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Degenerate neuronal systems sustaining cognitive functions , 2004, Journal of anatomy.

[8]  F. Dick,et al.  Voxel-based lesion–symptom mapping , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[9]  H. Damasio,et al.  Neuroanatomical correlates of the Benton Facial Recognition Test and Judgment of Line Orientation Test , 2009, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[10]  T. Grabowski,et al.  Disconnection's renaissance takes shape: Formal incorporation in group-level lesion studies , 2008, Cortex.

[11]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Modules and brain mapping , 2011, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[12]  R. J. Frank,et al.  Three-dimensional in vivo mapping of brain lesions in humans. , 1992, Archives of neurology.

[13]  D. Tranel,et al.  Behavioral patterns and lesion sites associated with impaired processing of lexical and conceptual knowledge of actions , 2012, Cortex.

[14]  Daniel Y. Kimberg,et al.  Inappropriate usage of the Brunner–Munzel test in recent voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping studies , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  Chris Rorden,et al.  The anatomy of spatial neglect based on voxelwise statistical analysis: a study of 140 patients. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[16]  Chris Rorden,et al.  Improving Lesion-Symptom Mapping , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[17]  V S Caviness,et al.  Anatomy of Stroke, Part II: Volumetric Characteristics With Implications for the Local Architecture of the Cerebral Perfusion System , 2002, Stroke.

[18]  A. Damasio,et al.  A neural basis for the retrieval of conceptual knowledge , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[19]  R. J. Frank,et al.  Brainvox: An Interactive, Multimodal Visualization and Analysis System for Neuroanatomical Imaging , 1997, NeuroImage.

[20]  Bruno Dubois,et al.  A novel approach to clinical–radiological correlations: Anatomo-Clinical Overlapping Maps (AnaCOM): Method and validation , 2007, NeuroImage.

[21]  Chris Rorden,et al.  An evaluation of traditional and novel tools for lesion behavior mapping , 2009, NeuroImage.

[22]  D. Neary Lesion Analysis in Neuropsychology , 1990 .

[23]  H. Damasio,et al.  Does the Clock Drawing Test have focal neuroanatomical correlates? , 2008, Neuropsychology.

[24]  R. Adolphs,et al.  A Role for Somatosensory Cortices in the Visual Recognition of Emotion as Revealed by Three-Dimensional Lesion Mapping , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  D. Tranel,et al.  Damage to the default mode network disrupts autobiographical memory retrieval. , 2015, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[26]  Marc Hauser,et al.  Damage to Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Impairs Judgment of Harmful Intent , 2010, Neuron.