One too many: predicting future alcohol consumption following heavy drinking.
暂无分享,去创建一个
Mark Muraven | R. L. Collins | Saul Shiffman | S. Shiffman | J. Paty | M. Muraven | Jean A Paty | R Lorraine Collins | Elizabeth T Morsheimer | E. T. Morsheimer | Mark Muraven | R. Collins
[1] S. Shiffman,et al. Ecological Momentary Assessment (Ema) in Behavioral Medicine , 1994 .
[2] G. Metalsky,et al. Hopelessness depression: A theory-based subtype of depression. , 1989 .
[3] S Shiffman,et al. A day at a time: predicting smoking lapse from daily urge. , 1997, Journal of abnormal psychology.
[4] R. L. Collins,et al. Drinking restraint and alcohol-related outcomes: exploring the contributions of beverage instructions, beverage content and self-monitoring. , 1996, Journal of studies on alcohol.
[5] R. Longabaugh,et al. Predictive validity of Marlatt's relapse taxonomy versus a more general relapse code. , 1996, Addiction.
[6] Megan E. Piper,et al. Addiction motivation reformulated: an affective processing model of negative reinforcement. , 2004, Psychological review.
[7] M L Selzer,et al. A self-administered Short Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test (SMAST). , 1975, Journal of studies on alcohol.
[8] S. Nolen-Hoeksema,et al. Rumination, Depression, and Alcohol Use: Tests of Gender Differences , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy.
[9] R. L. Collins,et al. Binge drinking among underage college students: a test of a restraint-based conceptualization of risk for alcohol abuse. , 2001, Psychology of addictive behaviors : journal of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors.
[10] I G Kreft,et al. The Effect of Different Forms of Centering in Hierarchical Linear Models. , 1995, Multivariate behavioral research.
[11] S. Nolen-Hoeksema,et al. Gender Differences in Depression , 2001 .
[12] S. Shiffman,et al. Patient non-compliance with paper diaries , 2002, BMJ : British Medical Journal.
[13] A. Isen,et al. Toward understanding the role of affect in cognition. , 1984 .
[14] S. Shiffman,et al. Introduction to the special section: Ecological momentary assessment in health psychology. , 1998 .
[15] J. Singer,et al. Applied Longitudinal Data Analysis , 2003 .
[16] L. Parkinson,et al. A comparison of four computer-based telephone interviewing methods: Getting answers to sensitive questions , 2002, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.
[17] S. Shiffman,et al. The morning after: limit violations and the self-regulation of alcohol consumption. , 2005, Psychology of addictive behaviors : journal of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors.
[18] S. Shiffman,et al. First lapses to smoking: within-subjects analysis of real-time reports. , 1996, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.
[19] Anthony S. Bryk,et al. Hierarchical Linear Models: Applications and Data Analysis Methods , 1992 .
[20] S. Fiske,et al. The Handbook of Social Psychology , 1935 .
[21] D. Watson,et al. Toward a consensual structure of mood. , 1985, Psychological bulletin.
[22] T. K. Srull,et al. Handbook of Social Cognition , 1993 .
[23] C Peterson,et al. Causal explanations as a risk factor for depression: theory and evidence. , 1984, Psychological review.
[24] J. Russell. A circumplex model of affect. , 1980 .
[25] M. L. Cooper,et al. Drinking to regulate positive and negative emotions: a motivational model of alcohol use. , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[26] D. A. Kenny,et al. Data analysis in social psychology. , 1998 .
[27] R. L. Collins,et al. Restraint and attributions: Evidence of the abstinence violation effect in alcohol consumption , 1991, Cognitive Therapy and Research.
[28] L. Bensley. Construct validity evidence for the interpretation of drinking restraint as a response conflict. , 1991, Addictive behaviors.
[29] R. Collins,et al. Affective and behavioral reactions to the violation of limits on alcohol consumption. , 1994, Journal of studies on alcohol.
[30] L. Sobell,et al. Heavy drinking and negative affective situations in a general population and a treatment sample: Alternative explanations. , 1995 .
[31] R. Vuchinich,et al. A comparison of alcoholics' and nonalcoholics' causal attributions for drinking behavior. , 1982, The American journal of drug and alcohol abuse.
[32] R. L. Collins,et al. Temptation, restriction, and the regulation of alcohol intake: validity and utility of the temptation and restraint inventory. , 2000, Journal of studies on alcohol.
[33] S. Shiffman,et al. Progression from a smoking lapse to relapse: prediction from abstinence violation effects, nicotine dependence, and lapse characteristics. , 1996, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.
[34] Attributional style and the generality of learned helplessness. , 1984 .
[35] R. J. Williams,et al. Drinking restraint versus alcohol expectancies: which is the better indicator of alcohol problems? , 2000, Journal of studies on alcohol.
[36] R. L. Collins,et al. Ecological momentary assessment in a behavioral drinking moderation training program. , 1998, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.
[37] A. Stone,et al. Strategies for analyzing ecological momentary assessment data. , 1998, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.
[38] G. Affleck,et al. Mood and alcohol consumption: an experience sampling test of the self-medication hypothesis. , 2000, Journal of abnormal psychology.
[39] R. L. Collins,et al. The Temptation and Restraint Inventory for measuring drinking restraint. , 1992, British journal of addiction.
[40] S. Raudenbush,et al. Effects of study duration, frequency of observation, and sample size on power in studies of group differences in polynomial change. , 2001, Psychological methods.