Fish intelligence, sentience and ethics
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Jane Van Lawick-Goodall,et al. Tool-Using in Primates and Other Vertebrates , 1971 .
[2] Juan Pedro Vargas,et al. Encoding of geometric and featural spatial information by goldfish (Carassius auratus). , 2004, Journal of comparative psychology.
[3] Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al. A geometric process for spatial reorientation in young children , 1994, Nature.
[4] W. Thorpe. Learning and instinct in animals , 1956 .
[5] Culum Brown,et al. Laterality Influences Schooling Position in Rainbowfish, Melanotaenia spp , 2013, PloS one.
[6] L. Rogers. Lateralization in vertebrates: Its early evolution, general pattern, and development , 2002 .
[7] Giorgio Vallortigara,et al. Arithmetic in newborn chicks , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[8] Á. Miklósi,et al. Comparative social cognition: what can dogs teach us? , 2004, Animal Behaviour.
[9] Culum Brown. Familiarity with the test environment improves escape responses in the crimson spotted rainbowfish, Melanotaenia duboulayi , 2001, Animal Cognition.
[10] Valeria Anna Sovrano,et al. Recognition of partly occluded objects by fish , 2007, Animal Cognition.
[11] M. Bitterman. THE EVOLUTION OF INTELLIGENCE. , 1965, Scientific American.
[12] K. Laland,et al. Who follows whom? Shoaling preferences and social learning of foraging information in guppies , 1998, Animal Behaviour.
[13] G. Vallortigara,et al. Brain asymmetry (animal). , 2011, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.
[14] Anil K. Seth,et al. Consciousness and Complexity , 2022 .
[15] Anne E. Magurran,et al. Familiarity in schooling fish: how long does it take to acquire? , 1997, Animal Behaviour.
[16] C. Hawryshyn,et al. Behavioural studies of fish vision: an analysis of visual capabilities , 1990 .
[17] D. Chivers,et al. Familiarity and shoal cohesion in fathead minnows (Pimephales promelas): implications for antipredator behaviour , 1995 .
[18] Robert W. Mitchell,et al. Self-Awareness in Animals and Humans: Developmental Perspectives , 1994 .
[19] T. Håstein,et al. Expanding the moral circle: farmed fish as objects of moral concern. , 2007, Diseases of aquatic organisms.
[20] Mark E. Bouton,et al. Context effects on conditioning, extinction, and reinstatement in an appetitive conditioning preparation , 1989 .
[21] T. Collett,et al. Animal Navigation: Path Integration, Visual Landmarks and Cognitive Maps , 2004, Current Biology.
[22] Basis for Cumulative Cultural Evolution in Chimpanzees: Social Learning of a More Efficient Tool-Use Technique , 2013, PloS one.
[23] G. Iwama. The welfare of fish. , 2007, Diseases of aquatic organisms.
[24] C. Heyes,et al. SOCIAL LEARNING IN ANIMALS: CATEGORIES AND MECHANISMS , 1994, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.
[25] P. Sandøe,et al. Philosophical Debate on the Nature of Well-Being: Implications for Animal Welfare , 2002, Animal Welfare.
[26] Valeria Anna Sovrano,et al. Modularity as a fish (Xenotoca eiseni) views it: conjoining geometric and nongeometric information for spatial reorientation. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[27] R. Solomon,et al. Traumatic avoidance learning: the principles of anxiety conservation and partial irreversibility. , 1954, Psychological review.
[28] Peter K. McGregor,et al. Know thine enemy: fighting fish gather information from observing conspecific interactions , 1998, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[29] M. Bekoff,et al. Reflections on animal selves. , 2004, Trends in ecology & evolution.
[30] V. A. Sovrano. Visual lateralization in response to familiar and unfamiliar stimuli in fish , 2004, Behavioural Brain Research.
[31] Tom G. Pottinger,et al. Current issues in fish welfare , 2006 .
[32] L. Sneddon. Cognition and Welfare , 2011 .
[33] R. Douglas,et al. The Visual System of Fish , 1990 .
[34] R. Thornhill,et al. Major histocompatibility complex genes, symmetry, and body scent attractiveness in men and women , 2003 .
[35] G. Roth,et al. Evolution of the brain and intelligence , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[36] John R. Krebs,et al. Time-and-place learning by garden warblers, Sylvia borin , 1989, Animal Behaviour.
[37] L. Aronson. FURTHER STUDIES ON ORIENTATION AND JUMPING BEHAVIOR IN THE GOBIID FISH, BATHYGOBIUS SOPORATOR , 1971, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[38] R. Byrne,et al. Machiavellian intelligence II : extensions and evaluations , 1997 .
[39] C. Johnston,et al. Sound Production in Pimephales notatus (Rafinesque) (Cyprinidae) , 2000, Copeia.
[40] C. Packer,et al. Roaring and numerical assessment in contests between groups of female lions, Panthera leo , 1994, Animal Behaviour.
[41] M. Milinski. The function of mate choice in sticklebacks: optimizing Mhc genetics , 2003 .
[42] V. Braithwaite,et al. Fish and welfare: do fish have the capacity for pain perception and suffering? , 2004, Animal Welfare.
[44] S. Östlund-Nilsson,et al. The artistic three-spined stickleback (Gasterosteous aculeatus) , 2003, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[45] Marco Dadda,et al. Illusory patterns are fishy for fish, too , 2013, Front. Neural Circuits.
[46] Dittman,et al. Homing in Pacific salmon: mechanisms and ecological basis , 1996, The Journal of experimental biology.
[47] I. Duncan,et al. The implications of cognitive processes for animal welfare. , 1991, Journal of animal science.
[48] R. Moccia,et al. An evaluation of current perspectives on consciousness and pain in fishes , 2004 .
[49] Culum Brown,et al. Laterality is linked to personality in the black-lined rainbowfish, Melanotaenia nigrans , 2014, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[50] K. Laland,et al. 11. Social Learning in Fishes , 2011 .
[51] Henrik Mouritsen,et al. Magnetoreception and its use in bird navigation , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[52] M. Bekoff. Animal Passions and Beastly Virtues: Reflections on Redecorating Nature , 2006 .
[53] N. Block. Evidence against epiphenomenalism , 1991, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[54] T. Bakker,et al. Kin discrimination in sticklebacks is mediated by social learning rather than innate recognition , 2007 .
[55] R. Bshary,et al. Cleaner fish Labroides dimidiatus manipulate client reef fish by providing tactile stimulation , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[56] H. Fricke. Öko‐Ethologie des monogamen Anemonenfisches Amphiprion bicinctus (Freiwasseruntersuchung aus dem Roten Meer) , 2010 .
[57] Culum Brown,et al. Predation risk and decision making in poeciliid prey , 2011 .
[58] Leanne Proops,et al. Cross-modal individual recognition in domestic horses (Equus caballus) extends to familiar humans , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[59] L. Dill. Refraction and the spitting behavior of the archerfish (Toxotes chatareus) , 1977, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[60] G. Rosenthal,et al. Female preference for swords in Xiphophorus helleri reflects a bias for large apparent size. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[61] Simon M. Reader,et al. Innovation and social learning: individual variation and brain evolution * , 2003 .
[62] Learning to outwit a competitor in mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus torquatus). , 1994 .
[63] B. Skinner,et al. The Behavior of Organisms: An Experimental Analysis , 2016 .
[64] K. Laland,et al. Social learning of a novel avoidance task in the guppy: conformity and social release , 2002, Animal Behaviour.
[65] A. Carpenter. Monkeys opening Oysters , 1887, Nature.
[66] A. Popper,et al. Structure–function relationships in fish otolith organs , 2000 .
[67] G. Vallortigara,et al. Population lateralisation and social behaviour: A study with 16 species of fish , 2000, Laterality.
[68] L A Dugatkin,et al. Reversal of female mate choice by copying in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata) , 1992, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[69] K. Laland,et al. Social intelligence, innovation, and enhanced brain size in primates , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[70] Z. Pylyshyn,et al. Why are small and large numbers enumerated differently? A limited-capacity preattentive stage in vision. , 1994, Psychological review.
[71] S E Antell,et al. Perception of numerical invariance in neonates. , 1983, Child development.
[72] C. Neumeyer. Wavelength discrimination in the goldfish , 1986, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.
[73] J. Panksepp. Affective consciousness: Core emotional feelings in animals and humans , 2005, Consciousness and Cognition.
[74] Friedrich Ladich,et al. Are hearing sensitivities of freshwater fish adapted to the ambient noise in their habitats? , 2005, Journal of Experimental Biology.
[75] K. Laland,et al. Fish Cognition and Behavior , 2011 .
[76] Peter T. Sherman,et al. Behavioral response to familiar and unfamiliar neighbors in a territorial cichlid, Neolamprologus pulcher , 2004, Ichthyological Research.
[77] C. Gallistel,et al. Nonverbal Counting in Humans: The Psychophysics of Number Representation , 1999 .
[78] Redouan Bshary,et al. Social cognition in fishes , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[79] E. Natoli,et al. Free-ranging dogs assess the quantity of opponents in intergroup conflicts , 2010, Animal Cognition.
[80] C. Hawryshyn,et al. Functional mapping of ultraviolet photosensitivity during metamorphic transitions in a salmonid fish, Oncorhynchus mykiss. , 2001, The Journal of experimental biology.
[81] S. Dehaene,et al. Representation of number in the brain. , 2009, Annual review of neuroscience.
[82] Euan M. Macphail,et al. The evolution of consciousness , 1998 .
[83] Maarten Kamermans,et al. Teleost polarization vision: how it might work and what it might be good for , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[84] S. G. Reebs. Time-place learning in golden shiners (Pisces: Cyprinidae) , 1996, Behavioural Processes.
[85] K. Laland,et al. Social enhancement and social inhibition of foraging behaviour in hatchery-reared Atlantic salmon , 2002 .
[86] Chris Venditti,et al. Human frontal lobes are not relatively large , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[87] M. Taborsky. Broodcare helpers in the cichlid fish Lamprologus brichardi: Their costs and benefits , 1984, Animal Behaviour.
[88] Joan B. Silk,et al. Male bonnet macaques use information about third-party rank relationships to recruit allies , 1999, Animal Behaviour.
[89] R. Hinde,et al. The Opening of Milk Bottles by Birds , 1952, Nature.
[90] L. M. Gómez-Laplaza,et al. Time–place learning in the cichlid angelfish, Pterophyllum scalare , 2005, Behavioural Processes.
[91] T. Hara. Olfaction and gustation in fish: an overview. , 1994, Acta physiologica Scandinavica.
[92] D. Reiss,et al. Mirror self-recognition in the bottlenose dolphin: A case of cognitive convergence , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[93] Valeria Anna Sovrano,et al. Perception of Subjective Contours in Fish , 2009, Perception.
[94] G. Gallup. Chimpanzees: Self-Recognition , 1970, Science.
[95] A. Jones,et al. Tool use in the tuskfish Choerodon schoenleinii? , 2011, Coral Reefs.
[96] R. Gerlai,et al. Spontaneous discrimination of small quantities: shoaling preferences in angelfish (Pterophyllum scalare) , 2011, Animal Cognition.
[97] L. Means,et al. Breakfast in the nook and dinner in the dining room: time-of-day discrimination in rats , 2000, Behavioural Processes.
[98] Anthony D. Hawkins,et al. Spawning Sounds of the Male Haddock, Melanogrammus aeglefinus , 2000, Environmental Biology of Fishes.
[99] M. Wullimann,et al. Connections of the ventral telencephalon (subpallium) in the zebrafish (Danio rerio) , 2004, Brain Research.
[100] J. Kieffer,et al. The role of learning in fish behaviour , 1992, Reviews in Fish Biology and Fisheries.
[101] Michael Balls,et al. The Unheeded Cry: Animal Consciousness, Animal Pain and Science , 1991 .
[102] Andrew R. Cossins,et al. Behavioural analysis of a nociceptive event in fish: Comparisons between three species demonstrate specific responses , 2008 .
[103] L. Sneddon. Pain Perception in Fish: Evidence and Implications for the Use of Fish , 2011 .
[104] J. J. Beukemaj. Acquired hook‐avoidance in the pike Esox lucius L. fished with artificial and natural baits , 1970 .
[105] Clive D. L. Wynne,et al. Can fish really feel pain , 2014 .
[106] S. Tebbich,et al. Cleaner fish Labroides dimidiatus recognise familiar clients , 2002, Animal Cognition.
[107] L. S. Demski. The Pallium and Mind/Behavior Relationships in Teleost Fishes , 2013, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.
[108] Lynne U. Sneddon,et al. The Evidence for Pain in Fish: The Use of Morphine as an Analgesic , 2003 .
[109] R. Byrne,et al. Animal Tool-Use , 2010, Current Biology.
[110] C. Nicol. Farm Animal Cognition , 1996 .
[111] R. Gerlai,et al. Can angelfish (Pterophyllum scalare) count? Discrimination between different shoal sizes follows Weber’s law , 2010, Animal Cognition.
[112] S. Coussi-Korbel. Learning to outwit a competitor in mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus torquatus). , 1994, Journal of comparative psychology.
[113] S. G. Reebs. Time–Place Learning Based on Food but not on Predation Risk in a Fish, the Inanga (Galaxias maculatus) , 1999 .
[114] A. Kamil,et al. Long-term spatial memory in clark's nutcracker, Nucifraga columbiana , 1992, Animal Behaviour.
[115] M. Alexander,et al. Principles of Neural Science , 1981 .
[116] Culum Brown,et al. Predator Recognition in Rainbowfish, Melanotaenia duboulayi, Embryos , 2013, PloS one.
[117] G. Hunt. Manufacture and use of hook-tools by New Caledonian crows , 1996, Nature.
[118] Andrew B. Nelson,et al. Learned recognition of novel predator odour by convict cichlid embryos , 2013, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[119] C. Neumeyer,et al. Perception of illusory surfaces and contours in goldfish , 2007, Visual Neuroscience.
[120] Brian Butterworth,et al. Evidence for Two Numerical Systems That Are Similar in Humans and Guppies , 2012, PloS one.
[121] Mike Hansell,et al. Setting tool use within the context of animal construction behaviour. , 2008, Trends in ecology & evolution.
[122] S. Shettleworth. Cognition, evolution, and behavior , 1998 .
[123] Douglas P. Chivers,et al. Learning about Danger: Chemical Alarm Cues and Threat‐Sensitive Assessment of Predation Risk by Fishes , 2011 .
[124] G. Rosenthal. Using Video Playback to Study Sexual Communication , 1999, Environmental Biology of Fishes.
[125] M E Bitterman,et al. The comparative analysis of learning. , 1975, Science.
[126] Culum Brown,et al. Site fidelity and homing behaviour in intertidal fishes , 2013 .
[127] J. G. Rumney,et al. Fish mucous cocoons: the ‘mosquito nets’ of the sea , 2011, Biology Letters.
[128] G. Huse,et al. The Role of Fish Learning Skills in Fisheries and Aquaculture , 2007 .
[129] M. Milinski. The function of mate choice in sticklebacks: optimizing Mhc genetics*: mate choice in sticklebacks , 2003 .
[130] Culum Brown,et al. Lateralization of Cognitive Functions in Fish , 2011 .
[131] Guy Wallis,et al. A Species of Reef Fish that Uses Ultraviolet Patterns for Covert Face Recognition , 2010, Current Biology.
[132] D. Kitchen. Alpha male black howler monkey responses to loud calls: effect of numeric odds, male companion behaviour and reproductive investment , 2004, Animal Behaviour.
[133] C. Mccrohan,et al. Effect of noxious stimulation upon antipredator responses and dominance status in rainbow trout , 2009, Animal Behaviour.
[134] F. Rodríguez,et al. Brain and Cognition in Teleost Fish , 2011 .
[135] Redouan Bshary,et al. Interspecific Communicative and Coordinated Hunting between Groupers and Giant Moray Eels in the Red Sea , 2006, PLoS biology.
[136] A. Magurran,et al. Benefits of familiarity persist after prolonged isolation in guppies , 2006 .
[137] Valeria Anna Sovrano,et al. Modularity and spatial reorientation in a simple mind: encoding of geometric and nongeometric properties of a spatial environment by fish , 2002, Cognition.
[138] G. Helfman,et al. Social transmission of behavioural traditions in a coral reef fish , 1984, Animal Behaviour.
[139] John C. Montgomery,et al. Structure and function of the vertebrate magnetic sense , 1997, Nature.
[140] T. Bakker,et al. Olfactory self-recognition in a cichlid fish , 2009, Animal Cognition.
[141] Marian Stamp Dawkins,et al. Evolution and Animal Welfare , 1998, The Quarterly Review of Biology.
[142] Horst Bleckmann,et al. The brain creates illusions not just for us: sharks (Chiloscyllium griseum) can “see the magic” as well , 2014, Front. Neural Circuits.
[143] R. McCauley,et al. Attraction of settlement-stage coral reef fishes to reef noise , 2004 .
[144] V. Braithwaite,et al. The role of learning in fish orientation , 2003 .
[145] Louis Lefebvre,et al. Tools and brains in birds , 2002 .
[146] R. Bshary. Machiavellian intelligence in fishes , 2007 .
[147] A. Magurran,et al. Learned predator recognition and antipredator responses in fishes , 2003 .
[148] Culum Brown,et al. Laterality influences cognitive performance in rainbowfish Melanotaenia duboulayi , 2014, Animal Cognition.
[149] C. Heyes. Imitation, culture and cognition , 1993, Animal Behaviour.
[150] P. Strata,et al. Cerebellum and emotional behavior , 2009, Neuroscience.
[151] G. Gerlach,et al. Kin recognition in zebrafish: a 24-hour window for olfactory imprinting , 2008, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[152] Culum Brown. cerebral lateralisation, “social constraints,” and coordinated anti-predator responses , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[153] A. Magurran,et al. Schooling preferences for familiar fish in the guppy, Poecilia reticulata , 1994 .
[154] Kathryn E. Arnold. Kin recognition in rainbowfish (Melanotaenia eachamensis): sex, sibs and shoaling , 2000, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[155] Karen Wynn,et al. Addition and subtraction by human infants , 1992, Nature.
[156] R. Bshary,et al. Fish cognition: a primate's eye view , 2002, Animal Cognition.
[157] J. Evans,et al. Ecology and Evolution of Poeciliid Fishes , 2011 .
[158] M. Hauser,et al. Can rhesus monkeys spontaneously subtract? , 2001, Cognition.
[159] J. Beaugrand,et al. Coherent use of information by hens observing their former dominant defeating or being defeated by a stranger , 1996, Behavioural Processes.
[160] Domenico Parisi,et al. The Evolution of Pain , 2007, ECAL.
[161] R. Hubrecht,et al. Animal Consciousness, Cognition and Welfare , 2001, Animal Welfare.
[162] K. Frisch. Über einen Schreckstoff der Fischhaut und seine biologische Bedeutung , 1942, Zeitschrift für vergleichende Physiologie.
[163] M. Gentle,et al. Animal Studies Repository Animal Studies Repository Novel Object Test: Examining Nociception and Fear in the Rainbow Trout , 2022 .
[164] C. Brown,et al. Examining the link between personality and laterality in a feral guppy Poecilia reticulata population. , 2013, Journal of fish biology.
[165] K. Laland,et al. Shoaling generates social learning of foraging information in guppies , 1997, Animal Behaviour.
[166] J. D. Rose,et al. The Neurobehavioral Nature of Fishes and the Question of Awareness and Pain , 2002 .
[167] Culum Brown. Tool Use in Fishes , 2012 .
[168] G. Vallortigara,et al. survival with an asymmetrical brain: advantages and disadvantages of cerebral lateralization , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[169] N. Newcombe,et al. Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation? squaring theory and evidence , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[170] Kevin Warburton,et al. Social Mechanisms Enhance Escape Responses in Shoals of Rainbowfish, Melanotaenia duboulayi , 2004, Environmental Biology of Fishes.
[171] V. Braithwaite,et al. Population variation in lateralized eye use in the poeciliid Brachyraphis episcopi , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[172] Fiery Cushman,et al. Evolving the Psychological Mechanisms for Cooperation , 2005 .
[173] E. Tolman. Cognitive maps in rats and men. , 1948, Psychological review.