Humans can perceive heading without visual path information.

It has previously been reported that humans can determine their direction of 3D translation (heading) from the 2D velocity field of retinal motion experienced during self-motion through a rigid environment, as is done by current computational models of visual heading estimation from optic flow. However, these claims were supported by studies that used stimuli that contained low rotational flow rates and/or additional visual cues beyond the velocity field or a task in which observers were asked to indicate their future trajectory of self-motion (path). Thus, previous conclusions about heading estimation have been confounded by the presence of other visual factors beyond the velocity field, by the use of a path-estimation task, or both. In particular, path estimation involves an exocentric computation with respect to an environmental reference, whereas heading estimation is an egocentric computation with respect to one's line of sight. Here, we use a heading-adjustment task to demonstrate that humans can precisely estimate their heading from the velocity field, independent of visual information about path, displacement, layout, or acceleration, with accuracy robust to rotation rates at least as high as 20 deg/s. Our findings show that instantaneous velocity-field information about heading is directly available for the visual control of locomotion and steering.

[1]  Leland S. Stone,et al.  Translation and Rotation Trade Off in Human Visual Heading Estimation , 1996 .

[2]  Markus Lappe,et al.  A Neural Network for the Processing of Optic Flow from Ego-Motion in Man and Higher Mammals , 1993, Neural Computation.

[3]  J. Gibson The perception of the visual world , 1951 .

[4]  H. C. Longuet-Higgins,et al.  The interpretation of a moving retinal image , 1980, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[5]  W. Warren,et al.  Perception of translational heading from optical flow. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  J A Perrone,et al.  Model for the computation of self-motion in biological systems. , 1992, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[7]  J. Perrone,et al.  Human Heading Estimation During Visually Simulated Curvilinear Motion , 1997, Vision Research.

[8]  O. Braddick,et al.  The temporal integration and resolution of velocity signals , 1991, Vision Research.

[9]  Ellen C. Hildreth,et al.  Recovering heading for visually-guided navigation , 1992, Vision Research.

[10]  Scott N. J. Watamaniuk,et al.  Temporal and spatial integration in dynamic random-dot stimuli , 1992, Vision Research.

[11]  Allan D. Jepson,et al.  Visual Perception of Three-Dimensional Motion , 1990, Neural Computation.

[12]  James A. Crowell,et al.  The perception of heading during eye movements , 1992, Nature.

[13]  Y. Aloimonos,et al.  Direct Perception of Three-Dimensional Motion from Patterns of Visual Motion , 1995, Science.

[14]  J H Rieger,et al.  Processing differential image motion. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[15]  James A. Crowell,et al.  Estimating heading during real and simulated eye movements , 1996, Vision Research.

[16]  A. V. van den Berg,et al.  Why two eyes are better than one for judgements of heading , 1994, Nature.

[17]  L. Kaufman,et al.  Handbook of perception and human performance , 1986 .

[18]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Humans combine the optic flow with static depth cues for robust perception of heading , 1994, Vision Research.

[19]  J H Rieger,et al.  Information in optical flows induced by curved paths of observation. , 1983, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[20]  John P. Wann,et al.  Why you should look where you are going , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  Sheryl M. Ehrlich,et al.  PII: S0042-6989(97)00427-6 , 1998 .

[22]  J. Perrone,et al.  A model of self-motion estimation within primate extrastriate visual cortex , 1994, Vision Research.

[23]  D Regan,et al.  How do we avoid confounding the direction we are looking and the direction we are moving? , 1982, Science.

[24]  W. Bialek,et al.  Time Course of Information about Motion Direction in Visual Area MT of Macaque Monkeys , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  A. V. van den Berg,et al.  Judgements of Heading , 1996, Vision Research.

[26]  Constance S. Royden,et al.  Mathematical analysis of motion-opponent mechanisms used in the determination of heading and depth. , 1997, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[27]  A. Watson Probability summation over time , 1979, Vision Research.

[28]  M K Kaiser,et al.  Visual acceleration detection: Effect of sign and motion orientation , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.

[29]  Paul A. Braren,et al.  Wayfinding on foot from information in retinal, not optical, flow. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[30]  J. Movshon,et al.  Adaptive Temporal Integration of Motion in Direction-Selective Neurons in Macaque Visual Cortex , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  Navigation C. Hildreth Recovering Heading for Visually-Guided , 1992 .

[32]  S. McKee,et al.  Sequential recruitment in the discrimination of velocity. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[33]  Daniel J. Hannon,et al.  Direction of self-motion is perceived from optical flow , 1988, Nature.

[34]  A. V. van den Berg Judgements of heading. , 1996, Vision research.

[35]  Li Li,et al.  Perception of heading during rotation: sufficiency of dense motion parallax and reference objects , 2000, Vision Research.

[36]  A. Watson,et al.  The optimal motion stimulus , 1995, Vision Research.

[37]  Berthold K. P. Horn,et al.  Passive navigation , 1982, Computer Vision Graphics and Image Processing.

[38]  William H. Warren,et al.  Optic flow is used to control human walking , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[39]  D. Burr Temporal summation of moving images by the human visual system , 1981, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[40]  Nicholas G. Hatsopoulos,et al.  On the sufficiency of the velocity field for perception of heading , 1991, Biological Cybernetics.

[41]  Constance S. Royden,et al.  Analysis of misperceived observer motion during simulated eye rotations , 1994, Vision Research.

[42]  T J Sejnowski,et al.  A Model for Encoding Multiple Object Motions and Self-Motion in Area MST of Primate Visual Cortex , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  J. Koenderink,et al.  Facts on optic flow , 1987, Biological Cybernetics.

[44]  J. H. Rieger,et al.  Human visual navigation in the presence of 3-D rotations , 1985, Biological Cybernetics.

[45]  A. Remole PERCEPTION WITH AN EYE FOR MOTION , 1987 .

[46]  Dana H. Ballard,et al.  Egomotion perception using visual tracking , 1991, Comput. Intell..