Development of Infant Reaching Strategies to Tactile Targets on the Face

Infant development of reaching to tactile targets on the skin has been studied little, despite its daily use during adaptive behaviors such as removing foreign stimuli or scratching an itch. We longitudinally examined the development of infant reaching strategies (from just under 2 to 11 months) approximately every other week with a vibrotactile stimulus applied to eight different locations on the face (left/right/center temple, left/right ear, left/right mouth corners, and chin). Successful reaching for the stimulus uses tactile input and proprioception to localize the target and move the hand to it. We studied the developmental progression of reaching and grasping strategies. As infants became older the likelihood of using the hand to reach to the target – versus touching the target with another body part or surface such as the upper arm or chair – increased. For trials where infants reached to the target with the hand, infants also refined their hand postures with age. As infants became older, they made fewer contacts with a closed fist or the dorsal part of the hand and more touches/grasps with the fingers or palm. Results suggest that during the first year infants become able to act more precisely on tactile targets on the face.

[1]  E. Thelen,et al.  The transition to reaching: mapping intention and intrinsic dynamics. , 1993, Child development.

[2]  C. Spence,et al.  Human infants’ ability to perceive touch in external space develops postnatally , 2015, Current Biology.

[3]  Edward C. Chao,et al.  Generalized Estimating Equations , 2003, Technometrics.

[4]  Andrew N. Meltzoff,et al.  Neural body maps in human infants: Somatotopic responses to tactile stimulation in 7-month-olds , 2015, NeuroImage.

[5]  M. MacKay-Lyons Central pattern generation of locomotion: a review of the evidence. , 2002, Physical therapy.

[6]  P. Rochat Object manipulation and exploration in 2- to 5-month-old infants , 1989 .

[7]  W. Dunn,et al.  Sensory processing in children with and without autism: a comparative study using the short sensory profile. , 2007, The American journal of occupational therapy : official publication of the American Occupational Therapy Association.

[8]  H. Bloch,et al.  Lateralization of the Approach Movement and the Prehension Movement in Infants from 4 to 7 Months , 1996 .

[9]  Emalie G. McMahon,et al.  The Embodied Origins of Infant Reaching: Implications for the Emergence of Eye-Hand Coordination , 2018 .

[10]  A. Bremner,et al.  The Neural Basis of Somatosensory Remapping Develops in Human Infancy , 2014, Current Biology.

[11]  Tobias Heed,et al.  Using time to investigate space: a review of tactile temporal order judgments as a window onto spatial processing in touch , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[12]  Jenni M. Karl,et al.  Independent development of the Reach and the Grasp in spontaneous self-touching by human infants in the first 6 months , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[13]  P. Rochat,et al.  Oropharyngeal Control of Hand-Mouth Coordination in Newborn Infants , 1988 .

[14]  J. Bruner,et al.  Visually Preadapted Constituents of Manipulatory Action , 1972, Perception.

[15]  H. Coslett,et al.  From maps to form to space: Touch and the body schema , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Reaching and grasping. Parallel specification of visuomotor channels. , 1996 .

[17]  O. I. Fukson,et al.  Adaptability of innate motor patterns and motor control mechanisms , 1986, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[18]  R. Provine,et al.  Crossing the midline: limits of early eye-hand behavior. , 1979, Child development.

[19]  R. K. Clifton,et al.  Infants' Perception of Auditory Space. , 1991 .

[20]  N. Berthier,et al.  Development of reaching in infancy , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  Joseph Hilbe Generalized Estimating Equations, Second Edition , 2012 .

[22]  P. L. Adams THE ORIGINS OF INTELLIGENCE IN CHILDREN , 1976 .

[23]  Robert J. Simas,et al.  Developmental Trajectories of Hand Movements in Typical Infants and Those at Risk of Developmental Disorders: An Observational Study of Kinematics during the First Year of Life , 2018, Front. Psychol..

[24]  Crossing the midline: limits of early eye-hand behavior. , 1979, Child development.

[25]  P. Haggard,et al.  More than skin deep: Body representation beyond primary somatosensory cortex , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[26]  Andrew N Meltzoff,et al.  Neural representations of the body in 60-day-old human infants. , 2018, Developmental science.

[27]  O. I. Fukson,et al.  The spinal frog takes into account the scheme of its body during the wiping reflex. , 1980, Science.

[28]  Herbert Nichols Localization of Touch. , 1896 .

[29]  J. Bruner Organization of early skilled action. , 1973, Child development.

[30]  L. Rönnqvist,et al.  Quantitative assessment of right and left reaching movements in infants: a longitudinal study from 6 to 36 months. , 2006, Developmental psychobiology.

[31]  W. Penfield,et al.  SOMATIC MOTOR AND SENSORY REPRESENTATION IN THE CEREBRAL CORTEX OF MAN AS STUDIED BY ELECTRICAL STIMULATION , 1937 .

[32]  G. Butterworth,et al.  The development of hand-mouth coordination in 2- to 5-month-old infants: Similarities with reaching and grasping , 1997 .

[33]  Matej Hoffmann,et al.  Development of reaching to the body in early infancy: From experiments to robotic models , 2017, 2017 Joint IEEE International Conference on Development and Learning and Epigenetic Robotics (ICDL-EpiRob).

[34]  A. Engel,et al.  Tactile remapping: from coordinate transformation to integration in sensorimotor processing , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[35]  R. Kristeva,et al.  Reticular activating system of a central pattern generator: premovement electrical potentials , 2013, Physiological reports.

[36]  Annette Karmiloff-Smith,et al.  Specialization of the motor system in infancy: from broad tuning to selectively specialized purposeful actions. , 2017, Developmental science.