Interlimb transfer of load compensation during rapid elbow joint movements

Previous research has shown that training of a novel task can improve subsequent performance in the opposite arm owing to anticipation of the previously learned task conditions. Interestingly, we recently reported preliminary evidence that such transfer might also include modulation of feedback-mediated responses. We now test interlimb transfer of load compensation responses, measured through kinematic and EMG recordings during rapid 20° elbow flexion movements. Two subject groups, LR and RL, each comprising six right-handed subjects, first performed using either the left (LR) or right (RL) arm, followed by opposite arm performance. After 30 trials of consistent performance, five random trials within a background of 50 trials were loaded with a 2-kg mass prior to the “go” signal. We compared load compensation responses for naïve performance with those following opposite arm exposure. Under naïve conditions, the resulting load compensation responses began about 50 ms following movement onset, and were substantially more effective for the nondominant arm. Opposite arm exposure substantially improved the accuracy of only dominant arm responses. This, however, did not occur through changes in the short latency components of the load compensation response. Instead, changes in muscle activities, associated with interlimb transfer, began some 150 ms following movement onset. We expect that these changes represent transfer in the “volitional” component of the load compensation response. Because the shorter latency response was unaffected by opposite arm exposure, modulation of this component likely requires prior experience with limb specific effectors.

[1]  J. Lackner,et al.  Motor adaptation to Coriolis force perturbations of reaching movements: endpoint but not trajectory adaptation transfers to the nonexposed arm. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  Otmar Bock,et al.  Early stages of load compensation in human aimed arm movements , 1993, Behavioural Brain Research.

[3]  A. Prochazka,et al.  Implications of positive feedback in the control of movement. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  A. Prochazka,et al.  Muscle spindle discharge in normal and obstructed movements. , 1979, The Journal of physiology.

[5]  J. Tanji,et al.  Reflex and intended responses in motor cortex pyramidal tract neurons of monkey. , 1976, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  Carlo J. De Luca,et al.  The Use of Surface Electromyography in Biomechanics , 1997 .

[7]  J. Tanji,et al.  Gating of motor cortex reflexes by prior instruction. , 1974, Brain research.

[8]  P. Hodges,et al.  A comparison of computer-based methods for the determination of onset of muscle contraction using electromyography. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[9]  Robert L. Sainburg,et al.  Mechanisms underlying interlimb transfer of visuomotor rotations , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  R. Sainburg Evidence for a dynamic-dominance hypothesis of handedness , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  C. Marsden,et al.  Interaction between the long-latency stretch reflex and voluntary electromyographic activity prior to a rapid voluntary motor reaction , 1983, Brain Research.

[12]  J. Cooke,et al.  Initial agonist burst is modified by perturbations preceding movement , 1986, Brain Research.

[13]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Some factors pertinent to the organization and control of arm movements , 1982, Brain Research.

[14]  Mark L. Latash,et al.  Mirror Writing: Learning, Transfer, and Implications for Internal Inverse Models. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.

[15]  R. Sainburg,et al.  Interlimb transfer of visuomotor rotations: independence of direction and final position information , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  A. Prochazka Proprioception during voluntary movement. , 1986, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[17]  Gregor Thut,et al.  Intermanual transfer of proximal and distal motor engrams in humans , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[18]  J. Houk An assessment of stretch reflex function. , 1976, Progress in brain research.

[19]  N. A. Borghese,et al.  Transient reversal of the stretch reflex in human arm muscles. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Learned dynamics of reaching movements generalize from dominant to nondominant arm. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  A. Prochazka,et al.  Positive force feedback control of muscles. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Effect of load disturbances during centrally initiated movements. , 1978, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  A. Prochazka Sensorimotor gain control: A basic strategy of motor systems? , 1989, Progress in Neurobiology.

[24]  Amy J. Bastian,et al.  Inter- and intra-limb generalization of adaptation during catching , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  A Prochazka,et al.  The role of sensory information in the guidance of voluntary movement: reflections on a symposium held at the 22nd annual meeting of the Society for Neuroscience. , 1994, Somatosensory & motor research.

[26]  M. Gorassini,et al.  Catching a ball: contributions of intrinsic muscle stiffness, reflexes, and higher order responses. , 1994, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[27]  G. Gottlieb,et al.  EMG responses to an unexpected load in fast movements are delayed with an increase in the expected movement time. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  C. Ghez,et al.  Spinal mechanisms of the functional stretch reflex , 1978, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  C D Marsden,et al.  Latency measurements compatible with a cortical pathway for the stretch reflex in man. , 1973, The Journal of physiology.

[30]  J. Cooke,et al.  Responses to force perturbations preceding voluntary human arm movements , 1981, Brain Research.

[31]  P J Bairstow,et al.  Bilateral transfer in tapping skill in the absence of peripheral information. , 1970, Journal of motor behavior.

[32]  J. Houk,et al.  Regulatory actions of human stretch reflex. , 1976, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  C. Marsden,et al.  Influence of voluntary intent on the human long-latency stretch reflex , 1980, Nature.

[34]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[35]  John C. Rothwell,et al.  What do reflex and voluntary mean? Modern views on an ancient debate , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[36]  C. Gielen,et al.  Long‐latency stretch reflexes as co‐ordinated functional responses in man. , 1988, The Journal of physiology.

[37]  O. Bock Load compensation in human goal-directed arm movements , 1990, Behavioural Brain Research.

[38]  H. Imamizu,et al.  The locus of visual-motor learning at the task or manipulator level: implications from intermanual transfer. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[39]  Robert L Sainburg,et al.  Nondominant arm advantages in load compensation during rapid elbow joint movements. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  G. Gottlieb,et al.  Electromyographic responses to an unexpected load in fast voluntary movements: descending regulation of segmental reflexes. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  C. Marsden,et al.  Stretch reflex and servo action in a variety of human muscles. , 1976, The Journal of physiology.

[42]  W. Chambers,et al.  The effect of postcentral cortical lesions on motor responses to sudden upper limb displacements in monkeys , 1975, Brain Research.

[43]  Eric A Roy,et al.  Interlimb Transfer after Adaptation to Visual Displacement: Patterns Predicted from the Functional Closeness of Limb Neural Control Centres , 1981, Perception.