The maturation of eye movement behavior: Scene viewing characteristics in children and adults

While the close link between eye movements and visual attention has often been demonstrated, recently distinct attentional modes have been associated with specific eye movement patterns. The ambient mode-serving the localization of objects and dominating early scene inspection-is expressed by short fixations and large saccade amplitudes. The focal mode-associated with the identification of object details and dominating later stages of scene exploration-is indicated by longer fixations embedded in short saccades. The relationship between these processing modes and eye movement characteristics has so far only been examined in adults. While studies in children revealed a maturation of oculomotor behavior up to adolescence, developmental aspects of the processing modes are still unknown. Here we explored these mechanisms by comparing eye movements during the inspection of naturalistic scenes. Therefore, gaze behavior from adults and children in four different age groups (2, 4-6, 6-8, 8-10years old) was examined. We found a general effect of age, revealing that with age fixation durations decrease and saccade amplitudes increase. However, in all age groups fixations were shorter and saccades were longer at the beginning of scene inspection but fixations became longer and saccades became shorter over time. While saliency influenced eye guidance in the two youngest groups over the full inspection period, for the older groups this influence was found only at the beginning of scene inspection. The results reveal indications for ambient and focal processing strategies for as early as 2 years of age.

[1]  Martin I. Sereno,et al.  Dorsal and Ventral Stream Activation and Object Recognition Performance in School-age Children , 2010 .

[2]  B. Velichkovsky,et al.  Distractor effect and saccade amplitudes: Further evidence on different modes of processing in free exploration of visual images , 2009 .

[3]  K. Velanova,et al.  Development of eye-movement control , 2008, Brain and Cognition.

[4]  Fernanda Ferreira,et al.  Scene Perception for Psycholinguists. , 2004 .

[5]  Eva Aring,et al.  Visual fixation development in children , 2007, Graefe's Archive for Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology.

[6]  Steven C. Dakin,et al.  Horizontal Information Drives the Behavioral Signatures of Face Processing , 2010, Front. Psychology.

[7]  Boris M. Velichkovsky,et al.  Towards an express-diagnostics for level of processing and hazard perception , 2002 .

[8]  Janette Atkinson,et al.  Dorsal and ventral stream sensitivity in normal development and hemiplegia , 2002, Neuroreport.

[9]  Ernst Martin,et al.  Dorsal stream development in motion and structure-from-motion perception , 2008, NeuroImage.

[10]  B. Ambridge,et al.  The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. , 2004, Developmental psychology.

[11]  Susana T. L. Chung,et al.  The dependence of crowding on flanker complexity and target-flanker similarity. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[12]  Joan Stiles,et al.  The effects of stimulus density on children’s analysis of hierarchical patterns , 2001 .

[13]  E. Vurpillot,et al.  The development of scanning strategies and their relation to visual differentiation. , 1968, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[14]  David Ingle,et al.  Locating and identifying: Two modes of visual processing , 1967 .

[15]  B. Velichkovsky,et al.  Two Visual Systems and Their Eye Movements: Evidence from Static and Dynamic Scene Perception , 2005 .

[16]  Vision Research , 1961, Nature.

[17]  J. Henderson Human gaze control during real-world scene perception , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[18]  Boris M. Velichkovsky,et al.  Visual fixations and level of attentional processing , 2000, ETRA.

[19]  D. Munoz,et al.  Age-related performance of human subjects on saccadic eye movement tasks , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  Martin J. Steinbach,et al.  Mixed pro and antisaccade performance in children and adults , 2009, Brain Research.

[21]  J. Sweeney,et al.  The Emergence of Collaborative Brain Function: fMRI Studies of the Development of Response Inhibition , 2004, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[22]  M. Corbetta,et al.  The Reorienting System of the Human Brain: From Environment to Theory of Mind , 2008, Neuron.

[23]  C. Trevarthen,et al.  Two mechanisms of vision in primates , 1968, Psychologische Forschung.

[24]  D. Ballard,et al.  Eye guidance in natural vision: reinterpreting salience. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[25]  Thierry Baccino,et al.  New insights into ambient and focal visual fixations using an automatic classification algorithm , 2011, i-Perception.

[26]  E. Mellet,et al.  First came the trees, then the forest: developmental changes during childhood in the processing of visual local-global patterns according to the meaningfulness of the stimuli. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[27]  Sonja Grün,et al.  Cross-frequency interaction of the eye-movement related LFP signals in V1 of freely viewing monkeys , 2012, Front. Syst. Neurosci..

[28]  D. Ingle Two visual mechanisms underlying the behavior of fish , 1967, Psychologische Forschung.

[29]  D. Lane,et al.  Auditory attention switching: a developmental study. , 1991, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[30]  J. Atkinson,et al.  Development of human visual function , 2011, Vision Research.

[31]  Michael D. Dodd,et al.  Examining the influence of task set on eye movements and fixations. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[32]  Benjamin W. Tatler,et al.  Systematic tendencies in scene viewing , 2008 .

[33]  S. Gathercole Cognitive approaches to the development of short-term memory , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[34]  P. Hancock,et al.  Looking at movies and cartoons: eye-tracking evidence from Williams syndrome and autism. , 2009, Journal of intellectual disability research : JIDR.

[35]  R. Walker,et al.  A model of saccade generation based on parallel processing and competitive inhibition , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[36]  C. Koch,et al.  A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attention , 2000, Vision Research.

[37]  F. C. Volkmann Human visual suppression , 1986, Vision Research.

[38]  Linda Lundström,et al.  The pupils and optical systems of gecko eyes. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[39]  S. Pickering,et al.  The development of visuo-spatial working memory , 2001, Memory.

[40]  Peter J. B. Hancock,et al.  Viewing it differently: Social scene perception in Williams syndrome and Autism , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[41]  Abel G. Oliva,et al.  Gist of a scene , 2005 .

[42]  Lihan Chen,et al.  Temporal characteristics of overt attentional behavior during category learning , 2012, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[43]  Christof Koch,et al.  Image Signature: Highlighting Sparse Salient Regions , 2012, IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence.

[44]  M. Goodale,et al.  Two visual systems re-viewed , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[45]  A. Chandna,et al.  Natural history of the development of visual acuity in infants , 1991, Eye.

[46]  M. Korkman,et al.  Differential Development of Attention and Executive Functions in 3- to 12-Year-Old Finnish Children , 2001, Developmental neuropsychology.

[47]  Daphne L. McCulloch,et al.  A VEP investigation of parallel visual pathway development in primary school age children , 2004, Documenta Ophthalmologica.

[48]  T. Paus,et al.  Development of an ability to maintain verbally instructed central gaze fixation studied in 8- to 10-year-old children. , 1990, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[49]  A. Roucoux,et al.  Development of fixation and pursuit eye movements in human infants , 1983, Behavioural Brain Research.

[50]  Zenzi M. Griffin,et al.  Why Look? Reasons for Eye Movements Related to Language Production. , 2004 .

[51]  J. Todd,et al.  The effects of viewing angle, camera angle, and sign of surface curvature on the perception of three-dimensional shape from texture. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[52]  D. Hubel,et al.  The role of fixational eye movements in visual perception , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[53]  J. Stiles,et al.  Children's analysis of hierarchical patterns: evidence from a similarity judgment task. , 1996, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[54]  Pietro Perona,et al.  Graph-Based Visual Saliency , 2006, NIPS.

[55]  P. König,et al.  Developmental Changes in Natural Viewing Behavior: Bottom-Up and Top-Down Differences between Children, Young Adults and Older Adults , 2010, Front. Psychology.

[56]  Roberto Bolzani,et al.  Fixation Stability in Normal Children , 2004, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[57]  Alexander C. Schütz,et al.  Eye movements and perception: a selective review. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[58]  Helen J. Neville,et al.  Selective auditory attention in 3- to 5-year-old children: An event-related potential study , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[59]  T. Banton,et al.  All Developmental Dyslexic Subtypes Display an Elevated Motion Coherence Threshold , 2001, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[60]  David Whitney,et al.  Neural correlates of coherent and biological motion perception in autism. , 2011, Developmental science.

[61]  J. Colombo,et al.  Individual differences in infant visual attention: are short lookers faster processors or feature processors? , 1991, Child development.

[62]  B. Velichkovsky,et al.  Time course of information processing during scene perception: The relationship between saccade amplitude and fixation duration , 2005 .

[63]  J. H. Bair Development of voluntary control. , 1901 .

[64]  Elizabeth L Irving,et al.  Horizontal saccade dynamics across the human life span. , 2006, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[65]  Iain D. Gilchrist,et al.  Visual correlates of fixation selection: effects of scale and time , 2005, Vision Research.

[66]  Daphne Maurer,et al.  Sensitivity to first- and second-order motion and form in children and adults , 2009, Vision Research.

[67]  N. Lazar,et al.  Maturation of cognitive processes from late childhood to adulthood. , 2004, Child development.

[68]  Antonio Torralba,et al.  Contextual guidance of eye movements and attention in real-world scenes: the role of global features in object search. , 2006, Psychological review.

[69]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Individual Differences in Infant Fixation Duration Relate to Attention and Behavioral Control in Childhood , 2014, Psychological science.

[70]  S. Pensiero,et al.  Saccadic eye movement conjugation in children , 1995, Vision Research.

[71]  江上 千代美 Developmental characteristics of visual cognitive function during childhood according to exploratory eye movements , 2009 .

[72]  Robert F. Asarnow,et al.  Continuity in cognitive abilities from infancy to 12 years of age , 1991 .

[73]  Christof Koch,et al.  Learning a saliency map using fixated locations in natural scenes. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[74]  Rudolf Groner,et al.  Attention and eye movement control: An overview , 2005, European archives of psychiatry and neurological sciences.

[75]  A. Weitzenhoffer,et al.  ATTENTION AND EYE MOVEMENTS , 1970, The Journal of nervous and mental disease.

[76]  B. Velichkovsky,et al.  Attentional dynamics during free picture viewing: Evidence from oculomotor behavior and electrocortical activity , 2013, Front. Syst. Neurosci..

[77]  J. Helmert,et al.  Visual Fixation Durations and Saccade Amplitudes: Shifting Relationship in a Variety of Conditions , 2008 .

[78]  G J Hitch,et al.  Visual working memory in young children , 1988, Memory & cognition.

[79]  P. König,et al.  Developmental changes in natural viewing behavior bottom-up and top-down differences between children, young adults , 2010 .

[80]  M A Just,et al.  A theory of reading: from eye fixations to comprehension. , 1980, Psychological review.

[81]  Michael C. Frank,et al.  Eye-movements reveal attention to social information in autism spectrum disorder , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[82]  A. Vinter,et al.  Children's spatial analysis of hierarchical patterns: construction and perception. , 2010, Developmental psychology.

[83]  B. Velichkovsky,et al.  Eye typing in application: A comparison of two interfacing systems with ALS patients , 2008 .

[84]  K. Hoffmann,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Saccadic Suppression , 2002, Science.

[85]  R. Dougherty,et al.  The maturation of form and motion perception in school age children , 2005, Vision Research.

[86]  O. Houdé,et al.  The Shift from Local to Global Visual Processing in 6-Year-Old Children Is Associated with Grey Matter Loss , 2011, PloS one.

[87]  Benjamin W Tatler,et al.  The central fixation bias in scene viewing: selecting an optimal viewing position independently of motor biases and image feature distributions. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[88]  C. Klein,et al.  Development of prosaccade and antisaccade task performance in participants aged 6 to 26 years. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[89]  K. Rayner The 35th Sir Frederick Bartlett Lecture: Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual search , 2009, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[90]  J. Atkinson,et al.  Normal and anomalous development of visual motion processing: motion coherence and ‘dorsal-stream vulnerability’ , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[91]  Michael L. Mack,et al.  Viewing task influences eye movement control during active scene perception. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[92]  K. Fukushima,et al.  Development of voluntary control of saccadic eye movements I. Age-related changes in normal children , 2000, Brain and Development.

[93]  J. Henderson Regarding Scenes , 2007 .