Electrophysiological measurement of the effect of inter-stimulus competition on early cortical stages of human vision

Competition between inputs in early visual cortex has been established as a key determinant in perception through decades of animal single cell and human fMRI research. We developed a novel ERP paradigm allowing this competition to be studied in humans, affording an opportunity to gain further insight into how competition is reflected at the neural level. Checkerboard stimuli were presented to elicit C1 (indexing processing in V1), C2 (hypothesized to reflect V1 after extrastriate feedback), and P1 (extrastriate) components. Stimuli were presented in three randomized conditions: single stimulus, near proximity pairs and far proximity pairs. Importantly, near stimuli (0.16° visual angle apart) were positioned to compete in primary visual cortex, whereas far stimuli (2° apart) were positioned to compete in extrastriate visual areas. As predicted, the degree and spatial range of competition increased from the C1 component to the C2 and P1 components. Specifically, competitive interactions in C1 amplitude were modest and present only for near-proximity pairs, whereas substantial competition was present for the P1, even for far-proximity pairs. To our knowledge, this is the first study to measure how competition unfolds over time in human visual cortex. Importantly, this method provides an empirical means of measuring competitive interactions at specific stages of visual processing, rendering it possible to rigorously test predictions about the effects of competition on perception, attention, and working memory.

[1]  Justin M. Ales,et al.  V1 is not uniquely identified by polarity reversals of responses to upper and lower visual field stimuli , 2010, NeuroImage.

[2]  N. Lavie Distracted and confused?: Selective attention under load , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[3]  Jack M. Loomis,et al.  Lateral masking in foveal and eccentric vision , 1978, Vision Research.

[4]  Donald C Hood,et al.  Conventional pattern-reversal VEPs are not equivalent to summed multifocal VEPs. , 2003, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[5]  M C FLOM,et al.  VISUAL RESOLUTION AND CONTOUR INTERACTION. , 1963, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[6]  R D Pascual-Marqui,et al.  Standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA): technical details. , 2002, Methods and findings in experimental and clinical pharmacology.

[7]  S. Klein,et al.  Suppressive and facilitatory spatial interactions in peripheral vision: peripheral crowding is neither size invariant nor simple contrast masking. , 2002, Journal of vision.

[8]  R. Desimone,et al.  Competitive Mechanisms Subserve Attention in Macaque Areas V2 and V4 , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  P. Nunez,et al.  Electric fields of the brain , 1981 .

[10]  Steven J Luck,et al.  Manipulation of orthogonal neural systems together in electrophysiological recordings: the MONSTER approach to simultaneous assessment of multiple neurocognitive dimensions. , 2012, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[11]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of spatial selective attention in areas V1, V2, and V4 of macaque visual cortex. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  Steven J. Luck,et al.  ERPLAB: an open-source toolbox for the analysis of event-related potentials , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[13]  F. Werblin Control of Retinal Sensitivity II. Lateral Interactions at the Outer Plexiform Layer , 1974 .

[14]  H. K. Hartline,et al.  INHIBITION IN THE EYE OF LIMULUS , 1956, The Journal of general physiology.

[15]  N. Lavie Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  D. Hubel,et al.  Uniformity of monkey striate cortex: A parallel relationship between field size, scatter, and magnification factor , 1974, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[17]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Cortical sources of the early components of the visual evoked potential , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[18]  Charles E. Schroeder,et al.  The cruciform model of striate generation of the early VEP, re-illustrated, not revoked: A reply to Ales et al. (2013) , 2013, NeuroImage.

[19]  S. Luck An Introduction to the Event-Related Potential Technique , 2005 .

[20]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[21]  Jeff Miller,et al.  Measurement of ERP latency differences: a comparison of single-participant and jackknife-based scoring methods. , 2008, Psychophysiology.

[22]  R. Desimone,et al.  Responses of Neurons in Inferior Temporal Cortex during Memory- Guided Visual Search , 1998 .

[23]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Modulation of sensory suppression: implications for receptive field sizes in the human visual cortex. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  P. Cook,et al.  Lateral inhibition in the inner retina is important for spatial tuning of ganglion cells , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[25]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Identification of early visual evoked potential generators by retinotopic and topographic analyses , 1994 .

[26]  Hillel Pratt,et al.  Sensory ERP Components , 2011 .

[27]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Mechanisms of directed attention in the human extrastriate cortex as revealed by functional MRI. , 1998, Science.

[28]  R. Desimone,et al.  Selective attention gates visual processing in the extrastriate cortex. , 1985, Science.

[29]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Attentional modulation of visual motion processing in cortical areas MT and MST , 1996, Nature.

[30]  S. Luck,et al.  A Common Neural Mechanism for Preventing and Terminating the Allocation of Attention , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  Raja Parasuraman,et al.  Dissociation of visual C1 and P1 components as a function of attentional load: An event-related potential study , 2010, Biological Psychology.

[32]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[33]  Kimron L. Shapiro,et al.  The role of biased competition in visual short-term memory , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  Charles E. Schroeder,et al.  What does polarity inversion of extrastriate activity tell us about striate contributions to the early VEP? A comment on Ales et al. (2010) , 2013, NeuroImage.

[35]  C. Gross,et al.  Visuotopic organization and extent of V3 and V4 of the macaque , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[36]  D. Jeffreys,et al.  Source locations of pattern-specific components of human visual evoked potentials. I. Component of striate cortical origin , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[37]  Victor A. F. Lamme,et al.  The spatiotemporal profile of cortical processing leading up to visual perception. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[38]  Mathew Alpern,et al.  The Additivity of Contrast in the Human Eye , 1959, The Journal of general physiology.

[39]  Justin M. Ales,et al.  On determining the intracranial sources of visual evoked potentials from scalp topography: A reply to Kelly et al. (this issue) , 2013, NeuroImage.

[40]  K. Fujii,et al.  Visualization for the analysis of fluid motion , 2005, J. Vis..